首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   171篇
  免费   7篇
  2024年   1篇
  2020年   5篇
  2019年   3篇
  2018年   3篇
  2017年   3篇
  2016年   10篇
  2015年   1篇
  2014年   4篇
  2013年   17篇
  2012年   11篇
  2011年   6篇
  2010年   4篇
  2009年   2篇
  2008年   10篇
  2007年   5篇
  2006年   3篇
  2005年   4篇
  2004年   8篇
  2003年   5篇
  2002年   3篇
  2001年   8篇
  2000年   4篇
  1999年   5篇
  1997年   1篇
  1995年   2篇
  1993年   1篇
  1992年   3篇
  1991年   3篇
  1989年   4篇
  1988年   3篇
  1987年   3篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   1篇
  1983年   2篇
  1982年   1篇
  1980年   1篇
  1979年   1篇
  1978年   3篇
  1977年   1篇
  1976年   4篇
  1975年   3篇
  1973年   3篇
  1972年   1篇
  1971年   2篇
  1970年   2篇
  1969年   1篇
  1968年   1篇
  1967年   1篇
排序方式: 共有178条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
121.
Human morality may be thought of as a negative feedback control system in which moral rules are reference values, and moral disapproval, blame, and punishment are forms of negative feedback given for violations of the moral rules. In such a system, if moral agents held each other accountable, moral norms would be enforced effectively. However, even a properly functioning social negative feedback system could not explain acts in which individual agents uphold moral rules in the face of contrary social pressure. Dr. Frances Kelsey, who withheld FDA approval for thalidomide against intense social pressure, is an example of the degree of individual moral autonomy possible in a hostile environment. Such extreme moral autonomy is possible only if there is internal, psychological negative feedback, in addition to external, social feedback. Such a cybernetic model of morality and moral autonomy is consistent with certain aspects of classical ethical theories.  相似文献   
122.
123.
124.
Past research has verified that observers assume that objects are reliably oriented with respect to a gravitationally centered coordinate system. Observers also appear to attend more to specific parts of objects, like faces, that typically are closer to the top. In the present work, we explored whether or not observers have a generic bias to view tops as being more salient than bottoms. In three experiments, observers indicated whether random shapes appeared to be more similar to comparison shapes that shared identical tops rather than bottoms. Observers exhibited a reliable tendency to match figures with similarly shaped tops. Matching choice was also a function of global shape attributes such as axis of elongation or size. The findings are consistent with the notion that, in nature, tops tend to be the most visible part and to provide the best information with respect to important aspects of objects such as animal intentionality and artifact functionality.  相似文献   
125.
Subjects were presented with a list of digits to alternate ears for serial recall. The list was followed by a suffix, a redundant acoustic event which did not have to be recalled. The suffix was presented either to one of the ears or binaurally. In all cases the suffix gave rise to a selective impairment of recall of the final items in the list. The results are interpreted as showing first that stimuli of the kind used are processed by simultaneously selecting both ears rather than by switching attention, and second that the site of the suffix effect is after the selection mechanism.  相似文献   
126.
It is proposed that many of the releasing or disinhibiting effects caused by models can be accounted for by information-processing mechanisms, without recourse to the concept of reinforcement. More specifically, it is suggested that the process of viewing a model's behavior involves the activation of an interpretive schema. This renders the information the schema incorporates more accessible for subsequent use. If the schema incorporates (or is closely related to) behavior-specifying information, that information becomes more accessible as well, thus making it more likely to influence overt behavior. Two studies are reported that assessed the plausibility of this reasoning. In Experiment 1 subjects who had viewed an aggressive model perceived greater hostility in the behavior of an ambiguous stimulus person that they subsequently encountered than did control subjects. This finding is consistent with the assumption that the model's behavior activated a conceptual schema for use in interpretation. In Experiment 2 subjects in whom an aggressive schema had been activated under a guise displayed greater aggression in their subsequent behavior in a different context than did control subjects. This finding is consistent with the assumption that activating the conceptual schema also activated behavioral information. The discussion centers on the implications of the findings.  相似文献   
127.
128.
CASPER is a psychometrics software package suitable for instructional and research applications with IBM-PC-compatible computers. CASPER lets the user simulate or directly enter psychometric data. Numerous statistical analyses, file handling procedures, and graphics are included Analyses include factor analysis, multiple regression, correlation/partial correlation, moments analysis, reliability analysis, and item analysis.  相似文献   
129.
Contour interaction as a function of retinal eccentricity   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
  相似文献   
130.
It is argued that lateral masking is a composite of several processes. These processes include response competition, distribution of attention, perceptual grouping, and feature (contour) interaction. Three experiments were carried out in an attempt to isolate some of the components. In the first two experiments, it was shown that feature interaction dominates at close spacing but other processes dominate at wider spacing. The third experiment showed that at least part of the effect of perceptual grouping appears to be information provided about target location.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号