全文获取类型
收费全文 | 32752篇 |
免费 | 631篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
33386篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 176篇 |
2019年 | 192篇 |
2018年 | 3700篇 |
2017年 | 3019篇 |
2016年 | 2472篇 |
2015年 | 404篇 |
2014年 | 329篇 |
2013年 | 1072篇 |
2012年 | 965篇 |
2011年 | 2818篇 |
2010年 | 2770篇 |
2009年 | 1710篇 |
2008年 | 2065篇 |
2007年 | 2492篇 |
2006年 | 348篇 |
2005年 | 496篇 |
2004年 | 435篇 |
2003年 | 356篇 |
2002年 | 318篇 |
2001年 | 479篇 |
2000年 | 459篇 |
1999年 | 316篇 |
1998年 | 161篇 |
1997年 | 122篇 |
1996年 | 129篇 |
1995年 | 116篇 |
1993年 | 124篇 |
1992年 | 232篇 |
1991年 | 240篇 |
1990年 | 216篇 |
1989年 | 214篇 |
1988年 | 216篇 |
1987年 | 206篇 |
1986年 | 182篇 |
1985年 | 186篇 |
1984年 | 168篇 |
1983年 | 121篇 |
1982年 | 130篇 |
1979年 | 160篇 |
1977年 | 107篇 |
1975年 | 146篇 |
1974年 | 163篇 |
1973年 | 187篇 |
1972年 | 157篇 |
1971年 | 144篇 |
1970年 | 125篇 |
1969年 | 135篇 |
1968年 | 178篇 |
1967年 | 132篇 |
1966年 | 142篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
51.
Defensive reactions to aid in the context of a close relationship were studied. Young adult siblings reported on their self-esteem, the quality of the sibling relationship, relevant demographic variables, and a variety of components of defensiveness to aid. Consistent with theory, the components of defensiveness were interrelated. A large percentage (52%) of the variance in defensiveness was accounted for by conflict between siblings, dominance relative to one's sibling, low global self-esteem, high self-esteem when comparing oneself with one's sibling, and low levels of feelings of entitlement. Siblings were least defensive in reaction to aid from older and female siblings. Corroboration from a subset of benefactor siblings was obtained. The results are discussed in relation to recent thinking about the nature of receiving support in close relationships and recipients' reactions to aid. 相似文献
52.
A brief, simple measure of different types of attributions for partner behavior was examined in 3 studies of married couples. Reliability was established by high internal consistency and test-retest correlations. Causal and responsibility attribution scores correlated with marital satisfaction, attributions for marital difficulties, and attributions for actual partner behaviors generated by spouses. Responsibility attributions were related to (a) reported anger in response to stimulus behaviors used in the measure and (b) the amount of anger displayed by wives during a problem-solving interaction with their partner. The extent to which husbands and wives whined during their discussion also correlated with their responsibility attributions. The results address several problems with existing assessments, and their implications for the measurement of attributions in marriage are discussed. 相似文献
53.
Individuals' failure to exercise actual control over an event might be compensated for by trying to bolster a generalized, subjective sense of control. Control might then be sought by undertaking acts the effect of which on the environment is illusory. This observation led to the hypothesis that stress, which undermines persons' sense of control, would engender illusory perceptions of controllability. The hypothesis was tested in 3 experiments that required Ss to choose between 2 gambling forms. Although the 2 forms were essentially identical, 1 was designed to instill an illusion of control. The results showed that highly stressed Ss, compared with those who experienced low stress, preferred gambling forms that heightened perceptions of controllability. 相似文献
54.
R C Barnett N L Marshall J D Singer 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1992,62(4):634-644
Are changes over time in the quality of a woman's job associated with changes in her psychological distress? Do family roles moderate these relationships? We addressed these questions using longitudinal data from a 2-year 3-wave study of a stratified random sample of 403 employed women who varied in occupation, race, partnership, and parental status. After estimating individual rates of change for each woman on each of the predictors and the outcome, we modeled the relationships between family role occupancy and change in job-role quality on the one hand, and change in psychological distress on the other. Among single women and women without children, as job-role quality declined, levels of psychological distress increased. Among partnered women and women with children, change in job-role quality was unrelated to change in psychological distress. 相似文献
55.
Darlene E. McCown Ph.D. R.N. Chandra Sharma M.S.N. R.N. 《Journal of religion and health》1992,31(1):31-40
Pastors' children have been thought to have more behavioral problems than other children since they face high expectations and are part of families that are often under public scrutiny. The purpose of this study was to describe the social competencies and behavioral problems of a national sample of pastors' children, aged 4–16, and to compare them to age and sex standardized norms. Data were obtained on 98 children from a random sample of 62 families, using the Child Behavior Checklist. Results indicate that scores for both boys and girls at each age grouping fell within the acceptable norms for both social competencies and behavioral problems. 相似文献
56.
57.
M N Mc Leod 《The Psychoanalytic quarterly》1992,61(1):37-64
Freud revised his theory of dreams significantly. First, he discovered the role of the id in dreaming. He hypothesized that the function of dreaming is to gratify an infantile sexual wish, in accordance with the pleasure principle, and thereby to preserve sleep. However, he learned later that anxiety generated by recent events plays a greater role in stimulating dream formation than he had thought and that some dreams actually generate anxiety. These clinical observations caused him, in large part, to revise his theories of dreaming, of anxiety, and of the mental apparatus. 相似文献
58.
N E Adler H P David B N Major S H Roth N F Russo G E Wyatt 《The American psychologist》1992,47(10):1194-1204
Psychological research is increasingly involved in debates regarding abortion. While recognizing the diversity of ethical and moral issues intertwined with abortion, the American Psychological Association (APA) has focused its involvement on psychological factors, most recently by appointing an expert panel to review the literature on psychological effects. This article notes the history of APA involvement and reports on the panel's conclusions. It presents evidence that abortion is not likely to be followed by severe psychological responses and that psychological aspects can best be understood within a framework of normal stress and coping rather than a model of psychopathology. Correlates of more negative responses following abortion are also discussed. 相似文献
59.
P D Blanck A S Bellack R L Rosnow M J Rotheram-Borus N R Schooler 《The American psychologist》1992,47(7):959-965
The primary responsibility of the American Psychological Association's (APA) Committee on Standards in Research (CSR) is to advise the APA on issues and standards related to the protection of human participants in psychological research. A related goal is to enhance the use of good ethical practices by APA members. The purpose of this article is to foster the view of research ethics not as an affront to the integrity of sound research, but as opportunities for scientific rewards, including increased understanding of the meaning of data, enhanced recruitment, and the inclusion of more representative samples. Three ethical practices are discussed as examples of this general premise: respect for confidentiality, use of debriefing, and assurance that participants are noncoerced volunteers. The Committee's intent is to promote consideration of these issues, not to promulgate specific guidelines or procedures. 相似文献
60.
A series of experiments investigated whether people could integrate nonspatial information about an object with their knowledge of the object's location in space. In Experiments 1 and 3, subjects learned the locations of cities on a fictitious road map; in Experiments 2, 4, and 5, subjects were already familiar with the locations of buildings on a campus. The subjects then learned facts about the cities on the maps or the buildings on the campus. The question of interest was whether or not these nonspatial facts would be integrated in memory with the spatial knowledge. After learning the facts, subjects were given a location-judgment test in which they had to decide whether an object was in one region of the space or another. Knowledge integration was assessed by comparing levels of performance in two conditions: (a) when a city or a building name was primed by a fact about a neighboring city or building, and (b) when a city or a building name was primed by a fact about a distant city or building. Results showed that responses in Condition a were faster or more accurate, or both faster and more accurate, than responses in Condition b. These results indicate that the spatial and nonspatial information were encoded in a common memory representation. 相似文献