首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   917篇
  免费   65篇
  2023年   7篇
  2022年   5篇
  2021年   5篇
  2020年   15篇
  2019年   22篇
  2018年   30篇
  2017年   42篇
  2016年   44篇
  2015年   26篇
  2014年   32篇
  2013年   107篇
  2012年   51篇
  2011年   40篇
  2010年   29篇
  2009年   35篇
  2008年   39篇
  2007年   52篇
  2006年   43篇
  2005年   29篇
  2004年   31篇
  2003年   26篇
  2002年   29篇
  2001年   18篇
  2000年   14篇
  1999年   13篇
  1998年   7篇
  1997年   12篇
  1996年   5篇
  1995年   11篇
  1994年   9篇
  1993年   5篇
  1992年   7篇
  1991年   13篇
  1990年   10篇
  1989年   6篇
  1988年   9篇
  1987年   8篇
  1986年   7篇
  1985年   8篇
  1984年   7篇
  1983年   8篇
  1982年   5篇
  1981年   4篇
  1980年   5篇
  1979年   9篇
  1977年   6篇
  1976年   6篇
  1975年   5篇
  1971年   4篇
  1968年   4篇
排序方式: 共有982条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
791.
Contrary to an earlier study, the accuracy of isometric force production is shown to fall under the influence of short-term motor memory, and in a manner quite similar to that previously observed for linear and rotary movement. Four paid male high school seniors pulled on a control stick with one of four designated target force bands. The target ranges were randomly varied in one phase of the experiment, and were the same for a series of consecutive responses in another. A red light came on after each response, and remained on for one of four durations; subjects were instructed not to respond while the light was on. Key variables accounting for the positive findings in the present experiment appear to be the availability of repetition (or massing), and the utilization of appropriate concurrent motor cues which are not supported by correlated visual cues.  相似文献   
792.
793.
Different groups of rats received Pavlovian aversive conditioning in which US-shock intensity (0.25–1.0 mA) and CS-US correlation (+, 0, ?) were factorially varied. Then, the CS was administered for each group contingent upon the reinforced response in an appetitive choice discrimination. Absolute and ratio measures of speed of running in the presence of the CS showed that, at the start of discrimination training, CS+ suppressed and CS- facilitated performance. However, later in training but prior to any evidence of choice learning, these speed effects reversed, with the magnitude of both the initial and reversed effects being a positive function of US intensity. Consistent with the reversal of speeds, associative (choice) measures showed that CS+ facilitated and CS- retarded discrimination learning relative to CSo and, within limits, these effects were also amplified by stronger USs. The findings suggest that a CS has both affective (motivational) and signaling (associative) functions and that both are influenced by US intensity; however, the CS's affective property is rapidly extinguished in the presence of a hedonicly different reinforcer, while leaving the CS's signaling property largely intact. Hence, the CS functions as a transformed signal for the new (appetitive) reinforcer and facilitates or retards learning by mediating the reinforcer's presence (CS+) or absence (CS-).  相似文献   
794.
A PDP-12 is used to conduct operant experiments in which peak force, duration, time integral of force, and interresponse time may each serve as the criterion response property for reinforcement, and all four properties are simultaneously recorded as dependent variables. Calibration, acquisition and control, and data analysis programs are described.  相似文献   
795.
Several previous studies have reported a correlation (typically around −.30) between standardized verbal ability test scores and name identity minus physical identity reaction times (NI-PI) in a letter-matching task. This correlation has usually been thought to reflect the mutual dependence of both measures on long-term memory access time. The present research was designed to assess Carroll's (1981) suggestions that: (a) NI — PI may not be the optimal formula for predicting standardized test scores from letter matching data, and (b) NI — PI may actually be related to the speed rather than the power component of standardized tests. Fifty-one subjects previously tested with a standardized reading test participated in letter-matching and word-reading tasks. The latter tasks required subjects to read words and specially constructed pseudowords. Power and speed measures were derived from the word-reading tasks and the standardized test on theoretical grounds (and supported by a principal components analysis). The results supported both of Carroll's contentions. NI — PI reaction time was related more to speed than power and the NI — PI statistic (as usually used) was not the optimum formula for predicting standardized test scores.  相似文献   
796.
The purpose of the present study was to examine the effectiveness of parent enhancement training in facilitating treatment and maintenance effects of a parent training program. Seventeen mothers and their clinic-referred noncompliant children were assigned to either a parent training alone group or a parent training plus parent enhancement therapy group. Fifteen mothers and their nonclinic children served as a quasi-control group. All clinic-referred mother-child dyads were treated individually by teaching the mother to reward compliance and other prosocial behavior and to use time-out for noncompliance. In addition, mothers in the parent training plus parent enhancement group also received treatment related to the following areas: parent's perception of their child's behavior, parent's personal adjustment, parent's marital adjustment, and parent's extrafamilial relationships. Assessment consisted of four home observations by independent observers prior to treatment, after treatment, and at a 2-month follow-up. The data indicated that the parent training plus parent enhancement therapy was more effective than parent training alone in changing child deviant behavior at posttreatment and in maintaining child compliance, child deviant behavior, parental rewards and parent contingent attention at follow-up. The control group did not change over the three assessment periods.  相似文献   
797.
Case studies of the highly successful socialization commonly evident in the development of mentally precocious children may furnish an excellent source of heuristic information on how early experience contributes to development, without down-grading our understanding of how phenotypical development is jointly determined by the cumulative interactions of genotypical and experiential processes. Both the investigation and case study literature on bright children suggest that exceptional early abilities typically flourish in highly intellectual families who early involve their children in rational communication with adults and who intensively stimulate them cognitively during early development. Two general parental strategies emerge from the case studies, one following deliberate, systematic instruction, commonly involving play, and the other applying stimulation incidentally to intensive, continuing interaction between caregiver and child. Both strategies appear to be highly flexible, interactive, and child-oriented, however, and both overlap in many ways, drawing on combinations of similar specific techniques and following a similar sequence in which interactions become increasingly, but never totally, endogenously regulated, as the child masters in exceptional ways high level sybmbolic skills (especially reading but often writing and math), complex problem solving skills, and vast bodies of knowledge. Much of the difference between the strategies can be attributed to differences in parental belief systems that alternatively stress hereditarian or environmental bases for development.  相似文献   
798.
We reviewed the extent and variety of functional assessment procedures used in behavioral intervention studies. The contents of six research journals dating from 1986 to 1992 were searched for articles describing single-subject intervention studies that targeted problematic social behaviors. Articles meeting this initial criteria were examined to determine if a functional assessment was conducted. If so, the various procedures comprising the functional assessment, types of interventions conducted, and demographic characteristics of study participants were recorded for each study. Results are compared to similar reviews of research dating from 1980–1985. Potential reasons for the paucity of studies incorporating functional assessment procedures are discussed. Finally, directions for future research and applied practice are provided.  相似文献   
799.
Differential hemispheric contributions to the perceptual phenomenon known as theMcGurk effect were examined in normal subjects, I callosotomy patient, and 4 patients with intractable epilepsy. Twenty-five right-handed subjects were more likely to demonstrate an influence of a mouthed ward on identification of a dubbed acoustic word when the speaker’s fag vase lateralized to the LVF as compared with the RVF. In contrast, display of printed response alternatives in the RVF elicited a greater percentage of McGurk responses than display in the LVF. Visual field differences were absent in a group of 15 left-handed subjects. These results suggest that in right-handers, the two hemispheres may make distinct contributions to the McGurk effect. The callosotomy patient demonstrated reliable McGurk effects, but at a lower rate than the normal subjects and the epileptic control subjects. These data support the view that both the right and left hemisphere can make significant contributions to the McGurk effect.  相似文献   
800.
Tim Maudlin 《Topoi》1995,14(1):7-15
The aim of this essay is to distinguish and analyze several difficulties confronting attempts to reconcile the fundamental quantum mechanical dynamics with Born's rule. It is shown that many of the proposed accounts of measurement fail at least one of the problems. In particular, only collapse theories and hidden variables theories have a chance of succeeding, and, of the latter, the modal interpretations fail. Any real solution demands new physics.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号