全文获取类型
收费全文 | 811篇 |
免费 | 9篇 |
专业分类
820篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 7篇 |
2022年 | 7篇 |
2021年 | 6篇 |
2020年 | 16篇 |
2019年 | 21篇 |
2018年 | 30篇 |
2017年 | 37篇 |
2016年 | 41篇 |
2015年 | 20篇 |
2014年 | 31篇 |
2013年 | 96篇 |
2012年 | 48篇 |
2011年 | 40篇 |
2010年 | 26篇 |
2009年 | 34篇 |
2008年 | 34篇 |
2007年 | 47篇 |
2006年 | 35篇 |
2005年 | 27篇 |
2004年 | 28篇 |
2003年 | 27篇 |
2002年 | 26篇 |
2001年 | 13篇 |
2000年 | 11篇 |
1999年 | 12篇 |
1998年 | 5篇 |
1997年 | 8篇 |
1996年 | 4篇 |
1995年 | 9篇 |
1994年 | 6篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1991年 | 5篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 3篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 5篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 4篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1972年 | 2篇 |
1967年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有820条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
711.
712.
713.
Meiser-Stedman R Smith P Glucksman E Yule W Dalgleish T 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2007,35(2):191-201
Examining parent-child agreement for Acute Stress Disorder (ASD) and Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) in children and
adolescents is essential for informing the assessment of trauma-exposed children, yet no studies have examined this relationship
using appropriate statistical techniques. Parent-child agreement for these disorders was examined by structured interview
in a prospective study of assault and motor vehicle accident (MVA) child survivors, assessed at 2–4 weeks and 6 months post-trauma.
Children were significantly more likely to meet criteria for ASD, as well as other ASD and PTSD symptom clusters, based on
their own report than on their parent’s report. Parent-child agreement for ASD was poor (Cohen’s κ = −.04), but fair for PTSD
(Cohen’s κ = .21). Agreement ranged widely for other emotional disorders (Cohen’s κ = −.07–.64), with generalised anxiety
disorder found to have superior parent-child agreement (when assessed by phi coefficients) relative to ASD and PTSD. The findings
support the need to directly interview children and adolescents, particularly for the early screening of posttraumatic stress,
and suggest that other anxiety disorders may have a clearer presentation post-trauma. 相似文献
714.
OBJECTIVE: To compare the frequency of nail biting in 4 settings (interventions) designed to elicit the functions of nail biting and to compare the results with a self-report questionnaire about the functions of nail biting. DESIGN: Randomised allocation of participants to order of conditions. SETTING: University Psychology Department. SUBJECTS: Forty undergraduates who reported biting their nails. INTERVENTIONS: Left alone (boredom), solving maths problems (frustration), reprimanded for nail biting (contingent attention), continuous conversation (noncontingent attention). MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES: Number of times the undergraduates bit their nails. RESULTS: Nail biting occurred most often in two conditions, boredom and frustration. CONCLUSION: Nail biting in young adults occurs as a result of boredom or working on difficult problems, which may reflect a particular emotional state. It occurs least often when people are engaged in social interaction or when they are reprimanded for the behavior. 相似文献
715.
Kirkpatrick T Joyce E Milton J Duggan C Tyrer P Rogers RD 《Journal of personality disorders》2007,21(3):243-261
Previous studies have identified neuropsychological deficits in individuals with antisocial personality disorder and/or psychopathy. Few studies have examined neuropsychological functioning in individuals with borderline personality disorder (BPD), and no studies have yet investigated cognitive and emotional function in male prisoners with BPD. In this study, we compared the risky decision-making of 17 participants with a history of serious violent or sexual offenses and a diagnosis of DSM-IV BPD with that of 17 participants with similar offending histories but personality disorders other than BPD. Those with BPD exhibited altered processing of information about potential losses (punishment) when the probability of gains (reward) was high; they also increased their choice of risky options even in circumstances where this was clearly avoidable. These data suggest that individuals with a diagnosis of BPD and a history of serious offenses have problems integrating different reinforcement signals when choosing between risky actions, perhaps reflecting corticolimbic dysfunction as an underlying mechanism in BPD. 相似文献
716.
This article argues that Bowers and Paternoster’s emphasis on a moral community marks an important step towards a more ethical and effective approach to anti-doping. However, it also argues that the authors’ proposed strategies undermine their stated goal of effectively engaging athletes as partners in anti-doping efforts and raise ethical concerns. Their proposed emphasis on exploiting shaming as a punishment and their general view of athletes as adversaries fosters mistrust between athletes and those who enforce the anti-doping rules. Instead, this article describes a model for empowering athletes as stakeholders in anti-doping policy as a means to provide anti-doping rules with increased moral legitimacy among athletes. 相似文献
717.
Three experiments are reported in which tip-of-the-tongue states (TOTSs) were induced in subjects by reading them pieces of item-specific information. In Experiments 1 and 2, subjects attempted to name famous people. These experiments showed that, in a TOTS, seeing a picture of the face of the target person did not facilitate naming, whereas the initials of the person's name did. In Experiment 3, a similar result was obtained with a landmark-naming task. The results of the experiments are discussed with reference to current models of memory structure and name retrieval. 相似文献
718.
Tim Maudlin 《Topoi》1995,14(1):7-15
The aim of this essay is to distinguish and analyze several difficulties confronting attempts to reconcile the fundamental quantum mechanical dynamics with Born's rule. It is shown that many of the proposed accounts of measurement fail at least one of the problems. In particular, only collapse theories and hidden variables theories have a chance of succeeding, and, of the latter, the modal interpretations fail. Any real solution demands new physics. 相似文献
719.
Marcus Nyström Diederick C. Niehorster Tim Cornelissen Henrik Garde 《Behavior research methods》2017,49(4):1310-1322
Technological advancements in combination with significant reductions in price have made it practically feasible to run experiments with multiple eye trackers. This enables new types of experiments with simultaneous recordings of eye movement data from several participants, which is of interest for researchers in, e.g., social and educational psychology. The Lund University Humanities Laboratory recently acquired 25 remote eye trackers, which are connected over a local wireless network. As a first step toward running experiments with this setup, demanding situations with real time sharing of gaze data were investigated in terms of network performance as well as clock and screen synchronization. Results show that data can be shared with a sufficiently low packet loss (0.1 %) and latency (M = 3 ms, M A D = 2 ms) across 8 eye trackers at a rate of 60 Hz. For a similar performance using 24 computers, the send rate needs to be reduced to 20 Hz. To help researchers conduct similar measurements on their own multi-eye-tracker setup, open source software written in Python and PsychoPy are provided. Part of the software contains a minimal working example to help researchers kick-start experiments with two or more eye trackers. 相似文献
720.
Previous research suggests an angry God image is a narrative schema predicting support for more punitive forms of criminal justice. However, this research has not explored the possibility that racialization may impact one's God image. We perform logistic regression on Wave V of the Baylor Religion Survey to examine the correlation between an angry God image and the belief that police shoot Blacks more often because Blacks are more violent than Whites (a context-specific form of cultural racism). Engaging critical insights from intersectionality theory, we also interact angry God image with both racialized identity and racialized religious tradition. Results suggest that the angry God schema is associated with this form of cultural racism for White people generally as well as White Evangelicals, yet for Black Protestants, belief in an angry God is associated with resistance against this type of cultural racism. 相似文献