首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   923篇
  免费   83篇
  2023年   8篇
  2022年   5篇
  2021年   9篇
  2020年   18篇
  2019年   24篇
  2018年   34篇
  2017年   39篇
  2016年   45篇
  2015年   23篇
  2014年   33篇
  2013年   103篇
  2012年   51篇
  2011年   41篇
  2010年   36篇
  2009年   33篇
  2008年   42篇
  2007年   56篇
  2006年   37篇
  2005年   32篇
  2004年   32篇
  2003年   32篇
  2002年   30篇
  2001年   16篇
  2000年   17篇
  1999年   16篇
  1998年   11篇
  1997年   9篇
  1996年   6篇
  1995年   11篇
  1994年   10篇
  1993年   8篇
  1992年   11篇
  1991年   11篇
  1990年   8篇
  1989年   7篇
  1988年   7篇
  1987年   8篇
  1986年   4篇
  1985年   6篇
  1984年   4篇
  1983年   8篇
  1982年   5篇
  1981年   5篇
  1979年   5篇
  1977年   5篇
  1976年   8篇
  1975年   4篇
  1971年   4篇
  1962年   3篇
  1958年   3篇
排序方式: 共有1006条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
971.
Campaigns have become popular in public health approaches to suicide prevention; however, limited empirical investigation of their impact on behavior has been conducted. To address this gap, utilization patterns of crisis support services associated with the Department of Veterans Affairs' Veterans Crisis Line (VCL) suicide prevention campaign were examined. Daily call data for the National Suicide Prevention Lifeline, VCL, and 1‐800‐SUICIDE were modeled using a novel semi‐varying coefficient method. Analyses reveal significant increases in call volume to both targeted and broad resources during the campaign. Findings underscore the need for further research to refine measurement of the effects of these suicide prevention efforts.  相似文献   
972.
973.
974.
975.
This study examines a form of masking that can take place when the signal and masker are widely separated in frequency and cannot be explained in terms of the traditional concept of the auditory filter or critical band. We refer to this as across-channel masking. The task of the subject was to detect an increment in modulation depth of a 1000-Hz sinusoidal carrier. The carrier could either be sinusoidally amplitude modulated or sinusoidally frequency modulated at a 10-Hz rate. Modulation increment thresholds of this “target” signal were measured for the target alone, and in the presence of two interfering sounds with carrier frequencies of 230 and 3300 Hz. When the interfering sounds were unmodulated, they had no effect on modulation increment thresholds. When the interfering sounds were either amplitude or frequency modulated, thresholds increased. Amplitude modulation (AM) increment thresholds were affected by both amplitude-modulated and frequency-modulated interference. Similarly, frequency modulation (FM) increment thresholds were affected by both amplitude-modulated and frequency-modulated interference. For both types of signal, the interference was tuned for modulation rate; across-channel masking was greatest when the interfering sounds were modulated at rates close to 10 Hz, and declined for higher or lower rates. However, the tuning was rather broad. When the target and interfering sounds were modulated at the same rate, there was no effect of the relative phase of the modulators. Two possible explanations for the results are discussed. One is based on the idea that carriers that are modulated in a similar way tend to be perceptually “grouped”. The other is based on the idea that there are “channels” in the auditory system tuned for AM and FM rate. Neither explanation appears completely satisfactory.  相似文献   
976.
977.
978.
979.
980.
The ability to inhibit an unfolding action is usually investigated using a stop signal (or gostop) task. The data from the stop-signal task are often described using a horse-race model whose key assumption is that each process (i.e., go, stop) exhibits stochastic independence. Using three variations of a coincident-timing task (i.e., go, gostop, and gostopgo) we extend previous considerations of stochastic independence by analysing the go latencies for prior effects of stopping. On random trials in the gostopgo task the signal sweep was paused for various times at various distances before the target. Significant increases in latency errors were reported on those trials on which the signal was paused (p <.005). Further analyses of the pause trials revealed significant effects for both the stopping interval (p <.001) and the pause interval (p <.05). Tukey post hoc analyses demonstrated increased latency errors as a linear function of the stopping interval, as expected, and decreased latency errors as a nonlinear function of the pause interval. These latter results indicate that the latencies of the go process, as reflected in the latency errors, may not exhibit stochastic independence under certain conditions. Various control mechanisms were considered in an attempt to explain these data.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号