全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4508篇 |
免费 | 63篇 |
专业分类
4571篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 54篇 |
2023年 | 56篇 |
2022年 | 46篇 |
2021年 | 53篇 |
2020年 | 177篇 |
2019年 | 90篇 |
2018年 | 158篇 |
2017年 | 218篇 |
2016年 | 240篇 |
2015年 | 187篇 |
2014年 | 218篇 |
2013年 | 728篇 |
2012年 | 207篇 |
2011年 | 180篇 |
2010年 | 138篇 |
2009年 | 169篇 |
2008年 | 167篇 |
2007年 | 159篇 |
2006年 | 135篇 |
2005年 | 77篇 |
2004年 | 84篇 |
2003年 | 95篇 |
2002年 | 65篇 |
2001年 | 40篇 |
2000年 | 74篇 |
1999年 | 40篇 |
1998年 | 32篇 |
1997年 | 23篇 |
1996年 | 56篇 |
1995年 | 38篇 |
1994年 | 35篇 |
1993年 | 39篇 |
1992年 | 46篇 |
1991年 | 47篇 |
1990年 | 38篇 |
1989年 | 26篇 |
1988年 | 22篇 |
1987年 | 19篇 |
1986年 | 17篇 |
1984年 | 12篇 |
1982年 | 19篇 |
1981年 | 13篇 |
1980年 | 21篇 |
1979年 | 24篇 |
1978年 | 26篇 |
1977年 | 27篇 |
1975年 | 11篇 |
1972年 | 10篇 |
1971年 | 12篇 |
1970年 | 12篇 |
排序方式: 共有4571条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
41.
Marek Franěk Jiří Mates Tomáš Radil Karin Beck Ernst Pöppel 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1991,49(6):509-516
Subjects (N = 32) were asked to synchronize a motor response with tones in auditory patterns. These patterns were created from six tones and six intertone intervals of equal duration. The pitch of the first tone differed from the others. It was found that subjects used three types of timing in their motor response: (1) the first intertone interval was prolonged and the second interval was shortened, (2) the second intertone interval was prolonged and the first interval was shortened, and/or (3) the first interval and the second interval were of approximately the same length. The prolongation of the fifth interval was observed during all three types of timing. The results are explained using the concept of suprasegmental control of timing, which explains a prolongation of intervals at critical control point of the patterns. The occurrence of three different strategies of timing is discussed in connection with similar principles in musical performance. 相似文献
42.
Summary An experiment was presented to test a functionalistic interpretation of the modality effect. This shows a superior recall performance for auditorily as opposed to visually presented verbal information. A total of 60 subjects were presented with mixed-mode (auditory-visual words), mixed-language (Swedish-English words), or mixed-category (category-unrelated words) lists, and were asked to recall the words of each list in any preferred order. The degree of organization according to modality, language, or category and the recall performance were measured. Organization by modality was significantly higher than organization by language or category as predicted by the functionalistic view proposed. The recall performance obtained for auditory and visual words differed in a way predicted by the functionalistic view. 相似文献
43.
Stanley Kaye PhD 《Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy》1982,13(2):175-181
Adoption of a child is an event in which a number of crises have converged. Both the biologic and adoptive families have contributed their failures and crises, and the subsequent interpretations of these by the adopted child can lead to polarized themes of goodness and badness. The absence of the biologic parents and of a coherent history reinforces the tendency to splitting. This paper explores these operations and raises questions about prevailing adoption practices.Paper presented at Spence-Chapin Symposium on Identity Problems of Children in Non-Nuclear Families, April 11, 1981. 相似文献
44.
The general psychophysical differential equation, dy/dx = W2(y)/W1(x), with the solution y = f(x), where x and y are subjective variables and W1 and W2 their subjective Weber functions, is (a) compared with a corresponding functional equation, and (b) studied from a stochastic point of view by error calculus, Methods for evaluating and handling divergences are proposed and illustrated for a number of combinations of Weber functions. It is shown that either the differential: and the functional equations have the same solution or the difference between the solutions is negligible compared to empirical scatter. The error calculus gives the same result: either no error at all or a negligible one. 相似文献
45.
Emory L. Cowen PhD Ellis L. Gesten Alice B. Wilson 《American journal of community psychology》1979,7(3):293-303
This study evaluated the effectiveness of the Primary Mental Project (PMHP), a program for early detection and prevention of school adjustment problems. Pre- and postprogram assessments were done with 215 primary-grade children seen in PMHP, usig teacher ratings of problem behaviors and competencies, and child-aide ratings of problems. School mental health professionals judged educational and behavioral changes in project children during the year. Significant across-the board improvements were found on all criterion measures. Modest intercorrelations among criterion change estimates suggested that the observed changes were due to program, rather than halo, effects. PMHP children also improved significantly more than matched, retrospective controls. 相似文献
46.
A microprocessor system can be used both to control aversive conditioning (classical, signaled and Sidman avoidance, and conflict) experiments and to acquire behavioral data in multiple subjects at a fraction of the cost of solid state systems. The software described in this paper can be used to program tones and shocks and count responses, independently, in up to 12 animals simultaneously. 相似文献
47.
48.
49.
Mary M. Mitchell PhD Michael Shayne Gallaway PhD MA Amy M. Millikan MD MPH Michael Bell MD MPH 《Suicide & life-threatening behavior》2012,42(5):486-494
Suicide is one of the leading causes of death among U.S. Army soldiers. Suicide‐related ideation, which is associated with suicide attempts and suicide, can cause considerable distress. In a sample of 1,663 recently redeployed soldiers, we used factor analysis and structural equation modeling to test the associations between combat exposure, unit cohesion, and their interaction in predicting suicide‐related ideation. We found that combat exposure was a significant risk factor for suicide‐related ideation, while unit cohesion was a significant protective factor. The significant interaction between the two factors indicated that soldiers who experienced greater combat exposure but also had higher levels of unit cohesion had relatively lower levels of suicide‐related ideation. In addition, those who had higher levels of combat exposure and lower unit cohesion were most at risk for suicide‐related ideation. Our findings indicate the importance of unit cohesion in protecting soldiers from suicide‐related ideation and suggest a higher risk group of soldiers who should be targeted for interventions. 相似文献
50.
Previous research has shown that in a three-term spatial reasoning task, the second premise of a German premise pair is especially easy to comprehend if (1) the prepositional object rather than the grammatical subject denotes the given entity, and if (2) the term denoting the given entity precedes the term denoting the new entity. Accordingly, the second premise is easiest to comprehend with noncanonical word order--that is, with the prepositional object in preverbal position denoting the given entity (e.g., To the right of the given object is the new subject). This finding is explained in terms of contextual licensing of noncanonical word order. Here, we discuss and tested two alternative accounts of contextual licensing, given-new and partially ordered set relations (Poset). The given-new account claims that noncanonical word order is licensed by the term denoting the given entity preceding the term denoting the new entity. On the Poset account, noncanonical word order is licensed if the preverbal constituent introduces a new entity that stands in a transitive, irreflexive, and asymmetric relation to a given entity. Comprehension times for second premises with spatial adverbs in four different word orders support both accounts of contextual licensing; Poset licensing was stronger than given-new licensing. 相似文献