全文获取类型
收费全文 | 10080篇 |
免费 | 144篇 |
专业分类
10224篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 50篇 |
2021年 | 117篇 |
2020年 | 172篇 |
2019年 | 165篇 |
2018年 | 247篇 |
2017年 | 244篇 |
2016年 | 325篇 |
2015年 | 211篇 |
2014年 | 283篇 |
2013年 | 986篇 |
2012年 | 472篇 |
2011年 | 484篇 |
2010年 | 307篇 |
2009年 | 283篇 |
2008年 | 427篇 |
2007年 | 414篇 |
2006年 | 354篇 |
2005年 | 350篇 |
2004年 | 351篇 |
2003年 | 322篇 |
2002年 | 307篇 |
2001年 | 184篇 |
2000年 | 137篇 |
1999年 | 157篇 |
1998年 | 137篇 |
1997年 | 142篇 |
1996年 | 118篇 |
1995年 | 118篇 |
1994年 | 101篇 |
1993年 | 113篇 |
1992年 | 121篇 |
1991年 | 112篇 |
1990年 | 100篇 |
1989年 | 68篇 |
1988年 | 78篇 |
1987年 | 73篇 |
1986年 | 74篇 |
1985年 | 91篇 |
1984年 | 90篇 |
1983年 | 88篇 |
1982年 | 86篇 |
1981年 | 84篇 |
1980年 | 78篇 |
1979年 | 82篇 |
1978年 | 79篇 |
1977年 | 87篇 |
1976年 | 68篇 |
1975年 | 64篇 |
1974年 | 81篇 |
1968年 | 53篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
171.
A sizeable amount of research literature has failed to demonstrate a stable relationship between self-report and the Rorschach (Exner, 1993). However, principal component first-factor related test-interaction style has been shown to moderate convergence. In this study 78 psychiatric patients completed the Rorschach and the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 (MMPI-2; Hathaway & McKinley, 1989). Practically no correlation was evident between the MMPI-2 and the Rorschach, measuring similar constructs, in all patients. Patients with similar test-interaction styles demonstrated positive intermethod correlations between both conceptually related and conceptually not directly related test indexes. The same scales were negatively correlated in patients with discordant test-interaction styles, and this difference between test-interaction style groups was significant. It is suggested that first-factor related test-interaction style moderates convergence. It is further suggested that test-interaction style moderates convergence between both conceptually related and conceptually not directly related measures of distress or psychopathology. 相似文献
172.
Employing fixed effects regression techniques on longitudinal data, we investigate how life events affect employees' job satisfaction. Unlike previous work–life research, exploring mostly contemporaneous correlations, we look for evidence of adaptation in the years following major life events. We find evidence of adaptation following the first marriage event, but we find that the birth of the first child has a long-lasting impact on employees' job satisfaction. Our findings also suggest that there is a general boost in job satisfaction prior to first marriage and to a lesser extent prior to the birth of the first child, consistent with evidence of anticipation. Accordingly, our study provides some of the first evidence on the dynamic effect of non-work related factors on job satisfaction and it introduces a novel methodology and a new perspective for investigating the dynamic interaction between the work and life domains. 相似文献
173.
This preliminary study investigated the feasibility of a brief Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) in a Swedish sample of unemployed individuals on long-term sick leave due to depression. Participants were randomized to a nonstandardized control condition (N = 16) or to the ACT condition (N = 18) consisting of 1 individual and 5 group sessions. From pretreatment to 18-month follow-up the ACT participants improved significantly on measures of depression, general health, and quality of life compared to participants in the control condition. The conditions did not differ regarding sick leave and employment status at any time point. The results indicate that ACT is a promising treatment for depression. The need for further refinements of future ACT protocols for this population is discussed. 相似文献
174.
175.
This study tested questions of ecological validity by comparing the eyewitness testimonies of children directly experiencing a painful inoculation experience those of children in a yoked-control group who vicariously experienced the inoculation onwith videotape. The study involved 86 5-year-olds, divided between 2 groups: the experiential and yoked control. The experiential group was followed through a health department with a video camera as they received diphtheria, pertussis, tetanus (DPT), and oral polio inoculations. They were tested immediately, 20 min later, and 1 month later. Each child in the yoked-control group merely watched the videotape of his or her counterpart in the experiential group, made similar ratings of pain, and was given the same tests and suggestions. Stress and personal experience affected items congruent with the stressor to produce flashbulb-like memories, with slower rates of forgetting for some items, such as nurse identifications, and greater suggestibility for other items, such as estimates of needle size. These and the apparently conflicting results in the literature were said to make sense when personally experienced stress was viewed from S.-A. Christianson's (1992) interactive perspective rather than as a single ubiquitous variable. 相似文献
176.
Arend W. A. Van Gemmert Hans-Leo Teulings George E. Stelmach 《Brain and cognition》2001,47(3):504-512
Parkinson's disease (PD) patients often show reductions in writing size (micrographia) as the length of the text they produce increases. The cause for these reductions in stroke size are not well understood. Reductions in stroke size could be associated with either concurrent processing demands that result from the coordination and control of fingers, wrist, and arm during writing and the processing of future words or increased extension of the wrist joint as the execution of the writing progresses to the right across the page, resulting in increased stiffness in the pen-limb system. Parkinson's patients and elderly controls wrote phrases of different lengths with target patterns in various serial positions. When the number of words to be written increased, PD patients reduced their stroke size of the initial target pattern, while the elderly controls did not reduce their stroke size. There was no systematic change in stroke size of the second pattern as function of serial position. This result suggests that PD patients reduce the size of their handwriting strokes when concurrent processing load increases. 相似文献
177.
Joel Thomas Tierno 《Sophia》2001,40(2):1-6
Those who advance the traditional argument from human freedom presume that human freedom provides an adequate explanation
of moral evil. I argue that this presumption is erroneous. An adequate explanation of our capacity to make choices that produce
moral evil must be distinguished from an adequate explanation of the actuality of such choices. Human freedom may account
for our ability to make choices that issue in moral evil. It cannot, by itself, account for our actually making such choices.
Something more than our potential for choices that produce moral evil is required to adequately explain the profusion of moral
evil that we actually find in the world. 相似文献
178.
J. Mark Thomas 《Zygon》2001,36(2):321-326
The spiritual situation at the turn of the millennium can be interpreted through Paul Tillich's appropriation of modernity, by analysis of the determinative structures and decisive trends of our age. The methods and organization of industry determine modern society. Spiritually, this situation results in the proliferation of means without ends, the objectification of natural structures, and the reduction of persons to things. Extrapolating from Tillich's analysis, the spiritual situation at the turn of the millennium can be understood as a quasi-religious struggle between the movements of liberal individualism and multiculturalism, both of which lose the sense of ambiguity. The communitarian movement and related interpretations remain the minority voice offering a mediating position. 相似文献
179.
Mark Thomas Walker 《Ratio》2001,14(1):68-83
I contend that while at least one of the arguments advanced by Bernard Williams in his paper 'Deciding To Believe' does establish that beliefs, or more precisely, judgements cannot be decided upon 'at will', the notion of truth-aimedness presupposed by that argument also, ironically, provides the key to understanding why judgements are necessarily voluntary. 相似文献
180.
Since the validity of Bell's inequalities implies the existence of joint probabilities for non-commuting observables, there is no universal consensus as to what the violation of these inequalities signifies. While the majority view is that the violation teaches us an important lesson about the possibility of explanations, if not about metaphysical issues, there is also a minimalist position claiming that the violation is to be expected from simple facts about probability theory. This minimalist position is backed by theorems due to A. Fine and I. Pitowsky.Our paper shows that the minimalist position cannot be sustained. To this end,we give a formally rigorous interpretation of joint probabilities in thecombined modal and spatiotemporal framework of `stochastic outcomes inbranching space-time' (SOBST) (Kowalski and Placek, 1999; Placek, 2000). We show in this framework that the claim that there can be no joint probabilities fornon-commuting observables is incorrect. The lesson from Fine's theorem is notthat Bell's inequalities will be violated anyhow, but that an adequate modelfor the Bell/Aspect experiment must not define global joint probabilities. Thus we investigate the class of stochastic hidden variable models, whichprima facie do not define such joint probabilities. The reasonwhy these models fail supports the majority view: Bell's inequalities are notjust a mathematical artifact. 相似文献