全文获取类型
收费全文 | 7056篇 |
免费 | 300篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
7358篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 85篇 |
2020年 | 128篇 |
2019年 | 112篇 |
2018年 | 174篇 |
2017年 | 163篇 |
2016年 | 208篇 |
2015年 | 157篇 |
2014年 | 191篇 |
2013年 | 669篇 |
2012年 | 334篇 |
2011年 | 339篇 |
2010年 | 217篇 |
2009年 | 201篇 |
2008年 | 295篇 |
2007年 | 308篇 |
2006年 | 255篇 |
2005年 | 268篇 |
2004年 | 257篇 |
2003年 | 237篇 |
2002年 | 244篇 |
2001年 | 123篇 |
2000年 | 84篇 |
1999年 | 117篇 |
1998年 | 103篇 |
1997年 | 104篇 |
1996年 | 86篇 |
1995年 | 79篇 |
1994年 | 74篇 |
1993年 | 79篇 |
1992年 | 89篇 |
1991年 | 81篇 |
1990年 | 68篇 |
1989年 | 50篇 |
1988年 | 48篇 |
1987年 | 57篇 |
1986年 | 60篇 |
1985年 | 72篇 |
1984年 | 66篇 |
1983年 | 70篇 |
1982年 | 63篇 |
1981年 | 75篇 |
1980年 | 68篇 |
1979年 | 62篇 |
1978年 | 68篇 |
1977年 | 71篇 |
1976年 | 65篇 |
1975年 | 54篇 |
1974年 | 61篇 |
1968年 | 40篇 |
1966年 | 43篇 |
排序方式: 共有7358条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
31.
The influence of alcohol and loud music on analytic versus holistic processing was examined in classification, concept learning, and embedded figures tasks. In the classification task, loud music reduced analytic responding by individuals who had consumed alcohol and increased analytic responding by individuals who had received placebo drinks. Alcohol also decreased the speed with which individuals were able to generate analytic responses in the classification task and decreased analytic performance on the embedded figures test. Alcohol slowed the rate of learning in the concept-learning task but did not lead to less analytic responding in that task. The results are consistent with the idea that alcohol slows the rate of information processing, particularly in tasks requiring visual analysis of stimuli. 相似文献
32.
Thomas G. Haring Blair Roger Mellanie Lee Catherine Breen Robert Gaylord-Ross 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1986,19(2):159-171
Three students with moderate handicaps were taught to initiate and expand on conversational topics. The teaching procedure used stimuli generated from actual conversations with nonhandicapped peers. Generalization was assessed by audiotaping conversations between the handicapped students and their peers in natural school contexts without adult supervision. Results indicated that training generalized to natural contexts. These results were socially validated by undergraduate special education students, who rated tapes of two of the students' conversations during training phases as more socially competent than during baseline. Results are discussed in terms of the evaluation of complex social behavior as multioperant behaviors. 相似文献
33.
As outlined by Snyder and Ickes (1985), the study of personality can be undertaken using one of three research approaches dispositional, situational, and interactive We show how the Social Relations Model provides an integrative method to estimate simultaneously dispositional, situational, and interactive effects Reviewed are component approaches to the study of personality The Social Relations Model is shown to be a component model (a special case of generalizability theory) applied in a social interaction context In the model, dispositional, situational, and interactive effects are termed actor, partner, and relationship effects, respectively The Social Relations Model can be used to answer a number of important issues in personality research The model can be used to assess reliability, measure the validity of self-ratings, and validate self-report inventories The model requires special designs in winch each person interacts with multiple partners Empirical examples are presented in which social anxiety, sex role inventories, and self-disclosure are studied 相似文献
34.
Thomas A. Hulbert Charles L. Gdowski David Lachar 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1986,14(1):115-122
Mothers' and fathers' responses on the Personality Inventory for Children (PIC) were compared for 360 clinic-referred children and adolescents. Interparent agreement was measured by several different indices. Correlations between parental pairs of scale T-scores derived from each parent averaged .66 for 13 of the profile scales; 9 of the scales exceeded this value. In contrast, mothers and fathers agreed in the classification of the presence of clinical significance an average of 77% of the time across these 13 scales, and pairs of parental PIC profiles contained an average of 3 scales in disagreement. The type of index used to measure interparent agreement was found to affect the results. The discussion examines the nature of interparent disagreement and addresses the implications regarding the use of fathers as informants on this instrument.This article is based on Thomas A. Hulbert's master's thesis. The authors extend their appreciation to James Orisell and Dena Mussaff for their assistance in data preparation and analysis. 相似文献
35.
Thomas A. Long 《Theoretical medicine and bioethics》1986,7(1):75-92
Alasdair MacIntyre's recent thinking both about the concept of a practice and the existence of narrative unity in human life raises important questions about how we should view clinical medicine today. Is it possible for clinical medicine to pursue patient well-being in a society (allegedly) afflicted with what he calls ‘modernity’? Here it is argued that MacIntyre's pessimistic view of the individual in contemporary society makes his call for patient autonomy in the clinical setting pointless. Finally, recent work in gerontology is cited to make three points: first, MacIntyre's pessimism about us is too extreme; second, the concept of a ‘fictionalized’ personal history is closer to reality than either MacIntyre's notion of narrative unity or the ideas of his imagined opponent (Sartre); and finally, we should not expect clinical medicine to produce patient well-being, when this is understood narratively. 相似文献
36.
37.
Thomas Tomlinson 《Theoretical medicine and bioethics》1986,7(2):105-121
Although the traditional physician ethic sees nothing objectionable about the doctor's influence over patients, superficial conceptions of the patient's right to self-determination imply that this influence may be manipulative. On the contrary, there are several different lines of argument which can reconcile self-determination with the physician's influence. Nevertheless, drawing the boundaries between legitimate methods of persuasion, and manipulation or coercion sometimes proves difficult. 相似文献
38.
Thomas M. Gruenenfelder 《Behavior research methods》1984,16(4):351-354
Although the typicality effect has been much studied in the semantic memory literature, typicality ratings exist for exemplars from only a very limited number of categories. This lack of ratings frequently limits the range of stimuli that can be used in investigations of the typicality effect. As an aid in stimulus construction, this paper reports typicality ratings of 893 exemplars from 93 different categories. 相似文献
39.
S. Stefan Soltysik Thomas Nicholas W. Jeffrey Wilson 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》1984,19(4):169-181
Respiratory rate, respiration amplitude, and vocal responses were recorded in cats of different ages during classic conditioning. Vocal responses to the conditional stimulus (CS) appeared first in 8-week-old kittens, and became prominent at older ages. An increase in respiration rate occurred after the onset of the CS in cats of all ages. Similarly, the decrease of respiration amplitude was observed at all ages, but only in 8-week-old and older subjects did this response resemble an adult pattern. In 4-week-old kittens the response was characterized by an early and brief peak, suggesting an alpha conditional response (CR). Clear and significant discrimination between a warning and a safety signal was present in both respiratory and vocal responses after the age of 8 weeks. 相似文献
40.