全文获取类型
收费全文 | 7404篇 |
免费 | 111篇 |
专业分类
7515篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 88篇 |
2020年 | 129篇 |
2019年 | 114篇 |
2018年 | 175篇 |
2017年 | 166篇 |
2016年 | 217篇 |
2015年 | 156篇 |
2014年 | 199篇 |
2013年 | 674篇 |
2012年 | 337篇 |
2011年 | 345篇 |
2010年 | 220篇 |
2009年 | 207篇 |
2008年 | 305篇 |
2007年 | 308篇 |
2006年 | 258篇 |
2005年 | 272篇 |
2004年 | 257篇 |
2003年 | 241篇 |
2002年 | 251篇 |
2001年 | 127篇 |
2000年 | 86篇 |
1999年 | 120篇 |
1998年 | 104篇 |
1997年 | 107篇 |
1996年 | 88篇 |
1995年 | 79篇 |
1994年 | 75篇 |
1993年 | 82篇 |
1992年 | 89篇 |
1991年 | 88篇 |
1990年 | 68篇 |
1989年 | 50篇 |
1988年 | 48篇 |
1987年 | 57篇 |
1986年 | 62篇 |
1985年 | 72篇 |
1984年 | 69篇 |
1983年 | 76篇 |
1982年 | 64篇 |
1981年 | 77篇 |
1980年 | 70篇 |
1979年 | 62篇 |
1978年 | 68篇 |
1977年 | 72篇 |
1976年 | 64篇 |
1975年 | 56篇 |
1974年 | 63篇 |
1968年 | 42篇 |
1966年 | 42篇 |
排序方式: 共有7515条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
211.
Thomas R. Zentall 《Current directions in psychological science》2000,9(4):118-123
The capacity for symbolic representation is a prerequisite for the development of human language because words, the basic units of language, are symbols that represent things. But symbolic representation may also serve a nonlinguistic role of organizing events into categories having the same meaning, and such a capacity could have considerable survival value for many species. In a number of experiments, my co-workers and I have found that pigeons that are trained to treat two different stimuli similarly also learn that those stimuli are commonly represented and, thus, that they have the same meaning. We have demonstrated evidence for such common representations in a number of ways, but perhaps the most convincing is when pigeons learn a new association involving one of the presumed commonly represented stimuli, and without further training demonstrate that they have learned a similar association involving the other stimulus. Furthermore, we have found that when pigeons are trained to treat two stimuli similarly, one of those stimuli is represented in terms of the other. These results have implications not only for the generality of cognitive processes across species, but also for the generality of symbolic representation beyond language use. 相似文献
212.
The Role of Families and Care Givers as Risk and Protective Factors in Preventing Youth Violence 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
This paper reviews research which discusses the risk and protective functions that families and other caregivers provide in influencing the development of aggressive behavior in youth. Currently, there is an emphasis on providing violence prevention programs in the school environment, typically with little parental or caregiver involvement. By enhancing the role of families and caregivers in youth violence prevention programs, we assert that an unique opportunity exists to both address specific risk factors for violence while enhancing the protective features of the family. Relatedly, the risk literature on youth violence indicates that the most influential risk factors (i.e., the family, community, and peers) have their principle impact on youth aggression outside the school. We suggest a shift in the focus of violence prevention programming that is more inclusive of families as both a risk and protective agent. In support of this position, relevant theory and reviews of exemplary family-involved programs are offered. Challenges to involving youth caregivers are identified and recommendations for overcoming those challenges suggested. Last, recommendations for future research and public policy in the prevention of youth violence are offered. 相似文献
213.
214.
Thomas R. Carretta Malcolm James Ree 《International Journal of Selection & Assessment》2000,8(4):227-236
Use of ability tests in personnel selection is addressed beginning with methodological issues. Studies are reviewed that show that almost all the validity of cognitive tests comes from general cognitive ability, g. Psychomotor ability is reviewed and found to have both higher- and lower-order factors, contrary to long-held beliefs. It was found that the higher-order general psychomotor factor was one source of validity of psychomotor tests. Additionally, psychomotor tests were shown to contain measures of g and to increment the validity of g-based measures very little. 相似文献
215.
216.
217.
218.
Thomas Mormann 《Journal for General Philosophy of Science》2009,40(1):155-160
219.
220.
Emily K. Farran;Harry R. M. Purser;Christopher Jarrold;Michael S. C. Thomas;Gaia Scerif;Vesna Stojanovik;Jo Van Herwegen; 《Developmental science》2024,27(1):e13421
Williams syndrome (WS) is a rare genetic syndrome. As with all rare syndromes, obtaining adequately powered sample sizes is a challenge. Here we present legacy data from seven UK labs, enabling the characterisation of cross-sectional and longitudinal developmental trajectories of verbal and non-verbal development in the largest sample of individuals with WS to-date. In Study 1, we report cross-sectional data between N = 102 and N = 209 children and adults with WS on measures of verbal and non-verbal ability. In Study 2, we report longitudinal data from N = 17 to N = 54 children and adults with WS who had been tested on at least three timepoints on these measures. Data support the WS characteristic cognitive profile of stronger verbal than non-verbal ability, and shallow developmental progression for both domains. Both cross-sectional and longitudinal data demonstrate steeper rates of development in the child participants than the adolescent and adults in our sample. Cross-sectional data indicate steeper development in verbal than non-verbal ability, and that individual differences in the discrepancy between verbal and non-verbal ability are largely accounted for by level of intellectual functioning. A diverging developmental discrepancy between verbal and non-verbal ability, whilst marginal, is not mirrored statistically in the longitudinal data. Cross-sectional and longitudinal data are discussed with reference to validating cross-sectional developmental patterns using longitudinal data and the importance of individual differences in understanding developmental progression. 相似文献