全文获取类型
收费全文 | 8251篇 |
免费 | 124篇 |
专业分类
8375篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 102篇 |
2020年 | 149篇 |
2019年 | 125篇 |
2018年 | 189篇 |
2017年 | 195篇 |
2016年 | 238篇 |
2015年 | 171篇 |
2014年 | 210篇 |
2013年 | 764篇 |
2012年 | 376篇 |
2011年 | 384篇 |
2010年 | 243篇 |
2009年 | 243篇 |
2008年 | 349篇 |
2007年 | 329篇 |
2006年 | 285篇 |
2005年 | 296篇 |
2004年 | 279篇 |
2003年 | 264篇 |
2002年 | 267篇 |
2001年 | 143篇 |
2000年 | 100篇 |
1999年 | 130篇 |
1998年 | 115篇 |
1997年 | 114篇 |
1996年 | 93篇 |
1995年 | 91篇 |
1994年 | 87篇 |
1993年 | 91篇 |
1992年 | 97篇 |
1991年 | 97篇 |
1990年 | 86篇 |
1989年 | 66篇 |
1988年 | 56篇 |
1987年 | 63篇 |
1986年 | 67篇 |
1985年 | 81篇 |
1984年 | 78篇 |
1983年 | 81篇 |
1982年 | 74篇 |
1981年 | 81篇 |
1980年 | 77篇 |
1979年 | 71篇 |
1978年 | 74篇 |
1977年 | 78篇 |
1976年 | 72篇 |
1975年 | 63篇 |
1974年 | 68篇 |
1973年 | 46篇 |
1968年 | 50篇 |
排序方式: 共有8375条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
991.
Thomas Furse 《Journal of the history of the behavioral sciences》2024,60(1):e22292
The US Army employed organizational and behavioral sciences in the context of the emerging Postindustrial political economy to shape its new strategic thought in the 1980s. This article examines how a group of military intellectuals in the Army applied ideas from these sciences to promote officer decision-making and decentralization while maintaining the Army's culture and ethics. They had significant reservations about bringing new ideas from the social sciences into the Army because Robert McNamara's modern cybernetic strategy had scarred the Army's morale and sense of self during the Vietnam War. Instead, the intellectuals carefully adapted ideas into the Army with an unsentimental attitude as it emerged from its post-Vietnam decline so it could fight complex maneuver warfare. Their strategic thought in the late Cold War made the Army a flexible global-spanning force for the unipolar moment in the 1990s and early 2000s. 相似文献
992.
The fall of Communism is now universally agreed to be what the philosopher Hegel called a world historical event—one that few predicted but nearly everyone saw as inevitable after it happened. In the aftermath many lives—and worldviews—changed, not only, but also in the human sciences. These remarks attempt to address in a preliminary way both the impact of the fall of Communism on psychology in former East Germany (including changes in personnel and approach) and the ways in which these sciences were employed as resources for reflection on the Communist past as well as the transition to new social and political regimes. 相似文献
993.
994.
995.
Xinyi Zhang Alexander English Thomas Talhelm Benjamin H. Nam Liuqing Wei 《Social and Personality Psychology Compass》2023,17(9):e12795
Cultures responded to the COVID-19 pandemic differently. We investigated cultural differences in mental health during the pandemic. We found regional differences in people's reports of anxiety in China over two years from 2020 to 2021 (N = 1186). People in areas with a history of rice farming reported more anxiety than people in wheat-farming areas. Next, we explored more proximal mechanisms that could help link the distal, historical factor of rice farming to people's modern experience of anxiety. Rice areas scored higher on collectivism and tight social norms than wheat areas, and collectivism, rather than norm tightness, mediated the rice-anxiety relationship. These findings advance our understanding of the distal sources of cultural differences, the proximal mechanisms, and mental health problems during the pandemics. 相似文献
996.
Andrea Trubanova Wieckowski Nicole N. Capriola-Hall Rebecca Elias Thomas H. Ollendick Susan W. White 《Cognition & emotion》2019,33(4):825-831
Prior research on attention bias in anxious youth, often utilising a visual dot probe task, has yielded inconsistent findings, which may be due to how bias is assessed and/or variability in the phenomenon. The present study utilises eye gaze tracking to assess attention bias in socially anxious adolescents, and explores several methodological and within-subject factors that may contribute to variability in attention bias. Attention bias to threat was measured in forty-two treatment-seeking adolescents (age 12–16 years) diagnosed with Social Anxiety Disorder. Bias scores toward emotional stimuli (vigilant attention) and bias scores away from emotional stimuli (avoidant attention) were explored. Bias scores changed between vigilance and avoidance within individuals and over the course of stimulus presentation. These differences were not associated with participant characteristics nor with self-reported social anxiety symptoms. However, clinician rated severity of social anxiety, explained a significant proportion of variance in the bias scores for adult, but not the adolescent, stimuli. Variability in attention bias among socially anxious adolescents is common and varies as a function of stimulus duration and type. Results may inform stimulus selection for future research. 相似文献
997.
Rachel Woodford Matthew J. Spittal Allison Milner Katie McGill Navneet Kapur Jane Pirkis Alex Mitchell Gregory Carter 《Suicide & life-threatening behavior》2019,49(1):23-40
Assessment of a patient after hospital‐treated self‐harm or psychiatric hospitalization often includes a risk assessment, resulting in a classification of high risk versus low risk for a future episode of self‐harm. Through systematic review and a series of meta‐analyses looking at unassisted clinician risk classification (eight studies; N = 22,499), we found pooled estimates for sensitivity 0.31 (95% CI: 0.18–0.50), specificity 0.85 (0.75–0.92), positive predictive value 0.22 (0.21–0.23), and negative predictive value 0.89 (0.86–0.92). Clinician classification was too inaccurate to be clinically useful. After‐care should therefore be allocated on the basis of a needs rather than risk assessment. 相似文献
998.
999.
Human communication is thoroughly context bound. We present two experiments investigating the importance of the shared context, that is, the amount of knowledge two interlocutors have in common, for the successful emergence and use of novel conventions. Using a referential communication task where black‐and‐white pictorial symbols are used to convey colors, pairs of participants build shared conventions peculiar to their dyad without experimenter feedback, relying purely on ostensive‐inferential communication. Both experiments demonstrate that access to the visual context promotes more successful communication. Importantly, success improves cumulatively, supporting the view that pairs establish conventional ways of using the symbols to communicate. Furthermore, Experiment 2 suggests that dyads with access to the visual context successfully adapt the conventions built for one color space to another color space, unlike dyads lacking it. In linking experimental pragmatics with language evolution, the study illustrates the benefits of exploring the emergence of linguistic conventions using an ostensive‐inferential model of communication. 相似文献
1000.
Jais Adam‐Troian Denis Caroti Thomas Arciszewski Tomas Sthl 《Applied cognitive psychology》2019,33(4):720-727
Previous research suggests that unfounded beliefs (UB)—such as conspiracist beliefs and beliefs in the supernatural—stem from similar cognitive and motivational mechanisms. More specifically, it has been demonstrated that cognitive ability is negatively associated with UB but only among individuals who value epistemic rationality. The present study goes beyond previous correlational studies by examining whether the negative association between cognitive ability and UB can be strengthened through a subtle rationality prime. In a large scale online experiment (N = 762 French teachers), we demonstrate that priming rationality (vs. control) does enhance the negative relationship between cognitive ability and adherence to supernatural beliefs, as well as conspiracy mentality (d = 0.2). This effect was not obtained for illusory pattern perception. This study's usefulness as a “proof of concept” for future interventions aimed at reducing UB prevalence among the general public is discussed. 相似文献