全文获取类型
收费全文 | 7403篇 |
免费 | 111篇 |
专业分类
7514篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 90篇 |
2020年 | 130篇 |
2019年 | 116篇 |
2018年 | 177篇 |
2017年 | 169篇 |
2016年 | 216篇 |
2015年 | 158篇 |
2014年 | 199篇 |
2013年 | 676篇 |
2012年 | 338篇 |
2011年 | 341篇 |
2010年 | 220篇 |
2009年 | 207篇 |
2008年 | 302篇 |
2007年 | 308篇 |
2006年 | 255篇 |
2005年 | 270篇 |
2004年 | 258篇 |
2003年 | 239篇 |
2002年 | 244篇 |
2001年 | 125篇 |
2000年 | 86篇 |
1999年 | 120篇 |
1998年 | 104篇 |
1997年 | 106篇 |
1996年 | 86篇 |
1995年 | 79篇 |
1994年 | 76篇 |
1993年 | 83篇 |
1992年 | 90篇 |
1991年 | 86篇 |
1990年 | 69篇 |
1989年 | 51篇 |
1988年 | 52篇 |
1987年 | 59篇 |
1986年 | 61篇 |
1985年 | 72篇 |
1984年 | 70篇 |
1983年 | 77篇 |
1982年 | 64篇 |
1981年 | 75篇 |
1980年 | 69篇 |
1979年 | 63篇 |
1978年 | 68篇 |
1977年 | 73篇 |
1976年 | 64篇 |
1975年 | 55篇 |
1974年 | 63篇 |
1968年 | 42篇 |
1966年 | 43篇 |
排序方式: 共有7514条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
821.
Two sources of attentional capture have been proposed: stimulus-driven (exogenous) and goal-oriented (endogenous). A resolution
between these modes of capture has not been straightforward. Even such a clearly exogenous event as the sudden onset of a
stimulus can be said to capture attention endogenously if observers operate in singleton-detection mode rather than feature-search
mode. In four experiments we show that a unique sudden onset captures attention even when observers are in feature-search
mode. The displays were rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) streams of differently coloured letters with the target letter
defined by a specific colour. Distractors were four #s, one of the target colour, surrounding one of the non-target letters.
Capture was substantially reduced when the onset of the distractor array was not unique because it was preceded by other sets
of four grey # arrays in the RSVP stream. This provides unambiguous evidence that attention can be captured both exogenously
and endogenously within a single task. 相似文献
822.
Mads Dyrholm Søren Kyllingsbæk Thomas Espeseth Claus Bundesen 《Journal of mathematical psychology》2011,55(6):416-429
We identify two biases in the traditional use of Bundesen’s Theory of Visual Attention (TVA) and show that they can be substantially reduced by introducing trial-by-trial variability in the model. We analyze whole and partial report data from a comprehensive empirical study with 347 participants and elaborate on Bayesian model selection theory for quantifying the advantage of trial-by-trial generalization in general. The analysis provides strong evidence of trial-by-trial variation in both the VSTM capacity parameter and perceptual threshold parameter of TVA. On average, the VSTM capacity bias was found to be at least half an item, while the perceptual threshold parameter was found to be underestimated by about 2 ms. 相似文献
823.
824.
The current investigation evaluated psychological and personality profiles of applicants to the diaconate and priesthood for several Episcopal dioceses. Applicants included both genders and their ages ranged from 29 to 67 years. A psychological testing battery including the MMPI-2, 16PF, and MCMI-III was administered to 42 applicants between 2008 and 2009 who subsequently entered the diaconate or priestly formation program in the Episcopal Church. Results indicate that these applicants were generally well-adjusted. Findings also suggest some tendency for defensiveness, repression, naïveté, and a strong need for affection, as well as for being emotionally stable, intelligent, trusting, and open to change. Finally, results suggest elevations on histrionic, narcissistic, and compulsive measures. 相似文献
825.
The distinction between data and phenomena introduced by Bogen and Woodward (Philosophical Review 97(3):303–352, 1988) was
meant to help accounting for scientific practice, especially in relation with scientific theory testing. Their article and
the subsequent discussion is primarily viewed as internal to philosophy of science. We shall argue that the data/phenomena
distinction can be used much more broadly in modelling processes in philosophy. 相似文献
826.
Studies of recognition typically involve tests in which the participant’s memory for a stimulus is directly questioned. There
are occasions however, in which memory occurs more spontaneously (e.g., an acquaintance seeming familiar out of context).
Spontaneous recognition was investigated in a novel paradigm involving study of pictures and words followed by recognition
judgments on stimuli with an old or new word superimposed over an old or new picture. Participants were instructed to make
their recognition decision on either the picture or word and to ignore the distracting stimulus. Spontaneous recognition was
measured as the influence of old vs. new distracters on target recognition. Across two experiments, older adults and younger
adults placed under divided-attention showed a greater tendency to spontaneously recognize old distracters as compared to
full-attention younger adults. The occurrence of spontaneous recognition is discussed in relation to ability to constrain
retrieval to goal-relevant information. 相似文献
827.
We provide an overview of three ways in which the expression “Historical epistemology” (HE) is often understood: (1) HE as
a study of the history of higher-order epistemic concepts such as objectivity, observation, experimentation, or probability;
(2) HE as a study of the historical trajectories of the objects of research, such as the electron, DNA, or phlogiston; (3)
HE as the long-term study of scientific developments. After laying out various ways in which these agendas touch on current
debates within both epistemology and philosophy of science (e.g., skepticism, realism, rationality of scientific change),
we conclude by highlighting three topics as especially worthy of further philosophical investigation. The first concerns the
methods, aims and systematic ambitions of the history of epistemology. The second concerns the ways in versions of HE can
be connected to versions of naturalized and social epistemologies. The third concerns the philosophy of history, and in particular
the level of analysis at which a historical analysis should aim. 相似文献
828.
829.
To alert professionals and consumers about safety risks associated with approved drugs, the U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA) periodically issues Drug Safety Communications, or DSCs (previously known as advisories, warnings, and health care professional letters). This review consolidates balanced information from 22 DSCs issued over the last 15 years by the FDA for drugs with pediatric indications (for any disorder) that are used to treat pediatric emotional and behavioral disorders (ADHD drugs, antipsychotics, antidepressants, and antiepileptics/anticonvulsants). A single-source document of pediatric DSCs for these drugs was needed because none existed previously; finding DSC information on the FDA website can be challenging; and other information sources (e.g., manufacturer or advocacy websites, blogs, other media reports) may lack the objectivity or accuracy that the FDA is charged to maintain. This consolidation is intended to enable better informed risk-benefit analysis around treatment selection and drug safety monitoring. For the 22 DSCs, we summarize the safety concerns, the populations affected, and when available from the FDA, the incidence of the adverse events, precursors, and factors that may increase or mitigate the risk of these very serious (e.g., sudden death, life-threatening rash, liver failure), but typically low incidence (<1 %) adverse events (cardiometabolic complications with atypical antipsychotics and suicidality with antidepressants are more common). This review does not address the far more common, but usually less serious, side effects that also accompany these drugs. Implications of this review for research and practice are discussed. 相似文献
830.