全文获取类型
收费全文 | 24079篇 |
免费 | 460篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
24540篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 250篇 |
2019年 | 341篇 |
2018年 | 442篇 |
2017年 | 397篇 |
2016年 | 482篇 |
2015年 | 333篇 |
2014年 | 461篇 |
2013年 | 1985篇 |
2012年 | 777篇 |
2011年 | 851篇 |
2010年 | 522篇 |
2009年 | 537篇 |
2008年 | 724篇 |
2007年 | 766篇 |
2006年 | 681篇 |
2005年 | 615篇 |
2004年 | 648篇 |
2003年 | 577篇 |
2002年 | 579篇 |
2001年 | 590篇 |
2000年 | 599篇 |
1999年 | 469篇 |
1998年 | 313篇 |
1997年 | 257篇 |
1996年 | 249篇 |
1995年 | 246篇 |
1992年 | 361篇 |
1991年 | 364篇 |
1990年 | 359篇 |
1989年 | 328篇 |
1988年 | 324篇 |
1987年 | 291篇 |
1986年 | 309篇 |
1985年 | 330篇 |
1984年 | 260篇 |
1983年 | 281篇 |
1982年 | 225篇 |
1981年 | 228篇 |
1979年 | 310篇 |
1978年 | 285篇 |
1977年 | 227篇 |
1975年 | 272篇 |
1974年 | 307篇 |
1973年 | 318篇 |
1972年 | 251篇 |
1971年 | 243篇 |
1969年 | 224篇 |
1968年 | 289篇 |
1967年 | 265篇 |
1966年 | 254篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
891.
892.
In our previous in vivo proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy ((1)H MRS) study we found reduced levels of N-acetylaspartate in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of chronic back pain patients. This study tests whether these chemical abnormalities can be detected in other pain states. Using (1)H MRS, we measured levels for N-acetylaspartate and other identifiable chemicals relative to creatine in four bilateral brain regions, including dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, cingulate, and thalamus, in a case of intractable severe sympathetically mediated chronic pain [complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS) type I]. The subject's chemical variations in the brain were compared to the same regional chemicals in 10 normal subjects (age- and sex-matched). Univariate statistics showed reduced levels of N-acetylaspartate in bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and increased levels of myo-inositol in left orbitofrontal cortex of the patient with intractable severe CRPS type I. These data support our original hypothesis that depletion of N-acetylaspartate in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is a chemical marker of chronic pain, indicating for neuronal degeneration. Unpredicted changes of orbitofrontal myo-inositol may be related to the specific mood/affective state in an extreme pain perception. This is the first report, which identifies chemical markers in the prefrontal cortex for objective measurement and monitoring of CRPS type I. This information might lead to valuable insights into diagnosis and future effective interventions of CRPS type I (e.g., prefrontal brain stimulation). 相似文献
893.
Papathomas TV 《Perception》2002,31(5):521-530
The English artist Patrick Hughes has created an extraordinary class of painted artpieces, most commonly referred to as 'reverspectives'. They consist of truncated pyramids and prisms with their smaller faces closer to the viewer, in such a way as to allow a realistic scene to be painted on them. The works of art contain rich perspective and other painted cues that conspire to elicit an illusory depth percept that is the reverse of the physical depth arrangement. This reverse depth is obtained under a wide range of viewing conditions, and competes with the veridical depth percept in a classical bistable paradigm that was found to exhibit a high degree of hysteresis. Under the illusory depth percept, reverspectives appear to move vividly as the viewer moves in front of them. This paper reports two experiments that were designed to assess the effectiveness of the painted cues in eliciting the illusory depth percept by using three different measures for the strength of the illusion. As expected, the illusion was favored by monocular viewing and large viewing distances. The results from these two experiments are in close agreement with each other, and they indicate that the painted cues are powerful in influencing the ultimate percept. 相似文献
894.
This study aimed to test the hypothesis that impairments of temporal duration processing after frontal lobe lesions reflect deficits in executive monitoring functions rather than a domain-specific deficit in the maintenance of duration information in working memory. Patients with frontodorsal lesions, clinical controls with post-central lesions, and healthy controls performed recognition and classification tasks, which should allow for testing maintenance and monitoring functions, respectively. Results showed mild non-selective impairments of the frontal patients on both temporal and spatial recognition tasks, but a marked selective degradation on temporal classification while performance on spatial classification was unimpaired. This suggests that maintenance of duration information in working memory after frontal lesions is basically preserved but that, depending on executive task characteristics, there is a specific deficit in the strategic organization of this type of information. 相似文献
895.
Functional magnetic resonance imaging has emerged as a powerful tool for mapping the neurologic underpinnings of sensory, motor and cognitive function. Much of this evolution carries assumptions about the subject population under study and, in particular, the neurologic status of subjects entered into studies either as healthy controls or as belonging to a specific disease group. Recent reports of incidental MRI abnormalities in normal volunteers for fMRI studies have brought to attention a variety of practical challenges and ethical dilemmas for researchers, many of whom are not physicians and most of whom have no formal radiological training. We propose a minimum standard for consenting subjects in fMRI protocols, and consider strategies over the longer term that call for expert physician participation, archiving of incidental findings including false positives, and the adoption of guidelines for handling variation in neural activations or performance that appear outside expected norms. 相似文献
896.
Many researchers have hypothesized relationships between personality disorders and gender role (i.e., masculinity and femininity). However, research has not addressed if people who are masculine or feminine more often meet the criteria for personality disorders. The present study examined whether college students (N = 665, 60% women) higher in masculinity or femininity more often exhibited features of the 10 DSM-IV personality disorders. Feminine men exhibited more features of all the personality disorders except antisocial. Dependent traits were associated with higher femininity and lower masculinity. Antisocial traits were associated with masculinity. Both men and women who typically behaved consistent with their gender had more narcissistic and histrionic features, whereas participants who typically behaved unlike their gender had more features of the Cluster A personality disorders. 相似文献
897.
The authors compare older adults' lexical-decision data with younger adults' data reported in P. Allen, A. F. Smith, et al. (2002). On the basis of their work, it was proposed that consistent-case wordswould be processed by the faster holistic (magnodominated) stream, but that mixed-case words would be processed by the slower analytic (interblob-dominated or blob-dominated) steams. Hue mixing was predicted to have no effect on consistent-case performance, but mixed-hue/mixed-case words were predicted to be recognized faster than monochrome/mixed-case words. Younger adults showed the predicted results, but older adults did not. These results suggest that holistic central processes are maintained, but that older adults exhibited an analytic decrement 相似文献
898.
When timing the mind one should also mind the timing: biases in the measurement of voluntary actions
Trevena and Miller (2002, this issue) provide further evidence that readiness potentials occur in the brain prior to the time that participants claim to have initiated a voluntary movement, a contention originally forwarded by Libet, Gleason, Wright, and Pearl (1983). In their examination of this issue, though, aspects of their data lead them to question whether their measurement of the initiation of a voluntary movement was accurate. The current article addresses this concern by providing a direct analysis of biases in this task. This was done by asking participants to make subjective timing decisions regarding a stimulus that could be measured objectively. Our findings suggest that their timing task was indeed biased such that participants' tend to report events as happening approximately 70 ms later than they actually happened. Implications for the original Libet et al. claims are discussed. 相似文献
899.
Induction of long-term synaptic changes at one synapse can facilitate the induction of long-term plasticity at another synapse. Here we show that if Aplysia sensory neuron (SN) somata and their remote motor neuron (MN) synapses are simultaneously exposed to serotonin (5HT) pulses, which at either site alone are insufficient to induce long-term facilitation (LTF), processes activated at these sites interact to induce LTF. Coincident induction of LTF requires: (1) that the synaptic pulse occurs within a brief temporal window of the somatic pulse and (2) that local protein synthesis occurs immediately at the synapse, followed by delayed protein synthesis at the soma. LTF at the SN-MN synapses can also be induced with cell-wide application of repeated pulses of 5HT. However, these two forms of LTF differ mechanistically: (1) coincident LTF requires protein synthesis in the postsynaptic motor neuron, whereas repeated 5HT LTF does not, and (2) repeated 5HT LTF is accompanied by intermediate-term (3 h) facilitation, whereas coincident LTF is not. Thus LTF expressed in the same temporal domain can result from different underlying mechanisms. 相似文献
900.
Waite TA 《Animal cognition》2002,5(4):209-214
Under the assumption that selection favors minimization of costly errors, erroneous choice may be common when its fitness
cost is low. According to an adaptive-choice model, this cost depends on the rate at which an animal encounters the choice:
the higher this rate, the smaller the cost of choosing a less valuable option. Errors should thus be more common when interruptions
to foraging are shorter. A previous experiment supported this prediction: gray jays, Perisoreus canadensis, were more error prone when subjected to shorter delays to access to food rewards. This pattern, though, is also predicted
by an attentional-constraints model. Because the subjects were able to inspect the rewards during delays, their improved performance
when subjected to longer delays could have been a byproduct of the experimentally prolonged opportunity for information processing.
To evaluate this possibility, a follow-up experiment manipulated both delay to access and whether rewards could be inspected
during delays. Depriving jays of the opportunity to inspect rewards (using opaque lids) induced only a small, nonsignificant
increase in error rate. This effect was independent of length of delay and so the jays' improved performance when subjected
to longer delays was not simply a byproduct of prolonged information processing. More definitively, even when the jays were
prevented from inspecting rewards during delays, their performance improved when subjected to longer delays. The findings
are thus consistent with the adaptive-choice model.
Electronic Publication 相似文献