首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   52532篇
  免费   1258篇
  国内免费   14篇
  2020年   353篇
  2019年   363篇
  2018年   3875篇
  2017年   3219篇
  2016年   2766篇
  2015年   613篇
  2014年   602篇
  2013年   2608篇
  2012年   1477篇
  2011年   3314篇
  2010年   3080篇
  2009年   2041篇
  2008年   2567篇
  2007年   2939篇
  2006年   853篇
  2005年   964篇
  2004年   910篇
  2003年   831篇
  2002年   801篇
  2001年   829篇
  2000年   803篇
  1999年   650篇
  1998年   411篇
  1997年   399篇
  1996年   385篇
  1992年   596篇
  1991年   571篇
  1990年   544篇
  1989年   494篇
  1988年   508篇
  1987年   484篇
  1986年   528篇
  1985年   517篇
  1984年   452篇
  1983年   413篇
  1981年   359篇
  1979年   486篇
  1978年   406篇
  1977年   366篇
  1976年   371篇
  1975年   411篇
  1974年   507篇
  1973年   521篇
  1972年   383篇
  1971年   413篇
  1970年   382篇
  1969年   404篇
  1968年   483篇
  1967年   444篇
  1966年   465篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
31.
32.
Human communication is thoroughly context bound. We present two experiments investigating the importance of the shared context, that is, the amount of knowledge two interlocutors have in common, for the successful emergence and use of novel conventions. Using a referential communication task where black‐and‐white pictorial symbols are used to convey colors, pairs of participants build shared conventions peculiar to their dyad without experimenter feedback, relying purely on ostensive‐inferential communication. Both experiments demonstrate that access to the visual context promotes more successful communication. Importantly, success improves cumulatively, supporting the view that pairs establish conventional ways of using the symbols to communicate. Furthermore, Experiment 2 suggests that dyads with access to the visual context successfully adapt the conventions built for one color space to another color space, unlike dyads lacking it. In linking experimental pragmatics with language evolution, the study illustrates the benefits of exploring the emergence of linguistic conventions using an ostensive‐inferential model of communication.  相似文献   
33.
34.
35.
36.
By roughly 6 years of age, children acquire the stereotype that men are more competent than women in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM), potentially leading to greater trust in scientific information provided by men. This study tested whether 3- to 8-year-old children differentially endorsed conflicting information about science and toys presented by male and female informants depicted as a ‘man’ and ‘woman’ (Exp1) or ‘scientists’ (Exp2). Children were expected to endorse toy testimony from gender-matched informants; thus, the key question concerned endorsement of science testimony. In Exp1 (N = 149), boys and girls showed a same-gender informant preference for toy testimony; however, girls endorsed the male informant's testimony more for science than for toys – but only when tested by a male experimenter. In Exp2 (N = 264), boys and girls showed a same-gender preference, irrespective of content. Findings suggest that STEM-related gender stereotypes might lead girls to trust scientific information presented by men over women in certain contexts.  相似文献   
37.
38.
A time-sharing paradigm was used to study potential interference effects of concurrent successive-auditory/vocal, successive-visual/motor, simultaneous-auditory/vocal, and simultaneous-visual/motor tasks on right- and left-handed manual-motor behaviors of differential levels of difficulty. Participants were selected from each of three developmental levels from 9 through 20 years of age. Results suggested that processing style (i.e., successive or simultaneous) interacts with modality (i.e., auditory/vocal or visual/motor) in terms of lateralized interference effects; however, potential effects due to age level were obfuscated by differential concurrent-task difficulties.  相似文献   
39.
40.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号