首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   32570篇
  免费   1139篇
  国内免费   14篇
  2020年   319篇
  2019年   365篇
  2018年   647篇
  2017年   639篇
  2016年   781篇
  2015年   553篇
  2014年   555篇
  2013年   2427篇
  2012年   1156篇
  2011年   1192篇
  2010年   607篇
  2009年   574篇
  2008年   804篇
  2007年   858篇
  2006年   741篇
  2005年   1924篇
  2004年   1279篇
  2003年   1032篇
  2002年   792篇
  2001年   883篇
  2000年   797篇
  1999年   662篇
  1998年   314篇
  1997年   293篇
  1996年   307篇
  1995年   275篇
  1992年   553篇
  1991年   550篇
  1990年   542篇
  1989年   482篇
  1988年   483篇
  1987年   452篇
  1986年   467篇
  1985年   480篇
  1984年   391篇
  1983年   353篇
  1981年   275篇
  1979年   439篇
  1978年   330篇
  1977年   286篇
  1975年   353篇
  1974年   438篇
  1973年   432篇
  1972年   327篇
  1971年   334篇
  1970年   324篇
  1969年   295篇
  1968年   400篇
  1967年   346篇
  1966年   338篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 281 毫秒
51.
52.
53.
54.
Brain lateralization research has led to speculation about counseling and guidance implications of left-right brain differences. Serious limitations in these implications are highlighted.  相似文献   
55.
Various lines of research have recently reported links between the ability to recall past and imagine future episodes. Here I report evidence that such links exist in development. Eighty‐two children were asked to report what they did yesterday and what they are going to do tomorrow. Children who could answer one were also more likely to answer the other question. Furthermore, there was an association between the quantity of responses for past and future questions. This correlation continued to be significant even when controlling for children's capacity to answer similar questions that did not have a temporal, episodic component (e.g., ‘tell me all the things that you can think of that are red’). Together, these results are in line with the growing evidence for fundamental links in the neuro‐cognitive resources involved in thinking about past and future events.  相似文献   
56.
57.
Developing readers often make anagrammatical errors (e.g. misreading pirates as parties), suggesting they use letter position flexibly during word recognition. However, while it is widely assumed that the occurrence of these errors decreases with increases in reading skill, empirical evidence to support this distinction is lacking. Accordingly, we compared the performance of developing child readers (aged 8–10 years) against the end‐state performance of skilled adult readers in a timed naming task, employing anagrams used previously in this area of research. Moreover, to explore the use of letter position by developing readers and skilled adult readers more fully, we used anagrams which, to form another word, required letter transpositions over only interior letter positions, or both interior and exterior letter positions. The patterns of effects across these two anagram types for the two groups of readers were very similar. In particular, both groups showed similarly slowed response times (and developing readers increased errors) for anagrams requiring only interior letter transpositions but not for anagrams that required exterior letter transpositions. This similarity in the naming performance of developing readers and skilled adult readers suggests that the end‐state skilled use of letter position is established earlier during reading development than is widely assumed.  相似文献   
58.
59.
60.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号