全文获取类型
收费全文 | 511篇 |
免费 | 10篇 |
专业分类
521篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 2篇 |
2023年 | 8篇 |
2022年 | 9篇 |
2021年 | 6篇 |
2020年 | 19篇 |
2019年 | 13篇 |
2018年 | 14篇 |
2017年 | 16篇 |
2016年 | 21篇 |
2015年 | 9篇 |
2014年 | 18篇 |
2013年 | 77篇 |
2012年 | 31篇 |
2011年 | 19篇 |
2010年 | 11篇 |
2009年 | 14篇 |
2008年 | 13篇 |
2007年 | 22篇 |
2006年 | 13篇 |
2005年 | 26篇 |
2004年 | 17篇 |
2003年 | 14篇 |
2002年 | 12篇 |
2001年 | 5篇 |
2000年 | 5篇 |
1999年 | 3篇 |
1998年 | 10篇 |
1997年 | 10篇 |
1996年 | 11篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1991年 | 5篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 4篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 6篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 3篇 |
1970年 | 2篇 |
1969年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 3篇 |
1967年 | 2篇 |
1966年 | 3篇 |
1954年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有521条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
51.
Lucian Gideon Conway III Laura Janelle Gornick Chelsea Burfeind Paul Mandella Andrea Kuenzli Shannon C. Houck Deven Theresa Fullerton 《Political psychology》2012,33(5):599-618
Research suggests that the integrative complexity of political rhetoric tends to drop during election season, but little research to date directly addresses if this drop in complexity serves to increase or decrease electoral success. The two present studies help fill this gap. Study 1 demonstrates that, during the Democratic Party primary debates in 2003–2004, the eventual winners of the party nomination showed a steeper drop in integrative complexity as the election season progressed than nonwinning candidates. Study 2 presents laboratory evidence from the most recent presidential campaign demonstrating that, while the complexity of Obama's rhetoric had little impact on college students' subsequent intentions to vote for him, the complexity of McCain's rhetoric was significantly positively correlated with their likelihood of voting for him. Taken together, this research is inconsistent with an unqualified simple is effective view of the complexity‐success relationship. Rather, it is more consistent with a compensatory view: Effective use of complexity (or simplicity) may compensate for perceived weaknesses. Thus, appropriately timed shifts in complexity levels, and/or violations of negative expectations relevant to complexity, may be an effective means of winning elections. Surprisingly, mere simplicity as such seems largely ineffective. 相似文献
52.
53.
Ford Julian D. Shevlin Mark Karatzias Thanos Charak Ruby Spinazzola Joseph 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2022,50(9):1207-1218
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology - Developmental Trauma Disorder (DTD) is a proposed child psychopathology diagnosis with emotion/somatic, attention/behavioral, and self/relational... 相似文献
54.
Ginny Russell Laura L. Miller Tamsin Ford Jean Golding 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2014,42(5):825-830
Retrospective recall about children’s symptoms is used to establish early developmental patterns in clinical practice and is also utilised in child psychopathology research. Some studies have indicated that the accuracy of retrospective recall is influenced by life events. Our hypothesis was that an intervention: speech and language therapy, would adversely affect the accuracy of parent recall of early concerns about their child’s speech and language development. Mothers (n?=?5,390) reported on their child’s speech development (child male to female ratio = 50:50) when their children were aged 18 or 30 months, and also reported on these early concerns retrospectively, 10 years later, when their children were 13 years old. Overall reliability of retrospective recall was good, 86 % of respondents accurately recalling their earlier concerns. As hypothesised, however, the speech and language intervention was strongly associated with inaccurate retrospective recall about concerns in the early years (Relative Risk Ratio = 19.03; 95 % CI:14.78–24.48). Attendance at speech therapy was associated with increased recall of concerns that were not reported at the time. The study suggests caution is required when interpreting retrospective reports of abnormal child development as recall may be influenced by intervening events. 相似文献
55.
Michael T. Ford John P. Agosta Jingyi Huang Choe Shannon 《Journal of business and psychology》2018,33(1):155-180
We propose a theoretical framework for and, via critical incidents, inductively investigate (a) the situations that trigger moral emotions toward others at work and (b) worker responses to these situations. Critical incidents were collected from a heterogeneous sample of 423 workers. Participants described an incident that caused each of four moral emotions (gratitude, anger, admiration, and contempt) and their response. Incidents and responses were coded, and frequencies, differences across the moral emotions, and associations between incidents and responses were analyzed. Several morally relevant situations at work were found to trigger the moral emotions. Participants responded in ways that had implications for their performance and their own well-being. The incidents largely align with moral foundations theory (Haidt and Graham in Social Justice Research, 20, 98–116, 2007). Responses also show evidence for social exchange, social learning, and rational expectancy-based processes and behavior. The incidents specify several practices for managers and organizations to encourage and avoid. This was the first qualitative study of moral emotions at work and one of the few workplace studies to examine these emotions empirically. Several understudied factors and behavioral responses emerged. 相似文献
56.
57.
58.
Linet R. Durmuşoğlu Sarah L. de Lange Theresa Kuhn Wouter van der Brug 《Political psychology》2023,44(3):583-601
Research shows that parents have a strong influence on the party preferences of their children. Yet little is known about how such preferences are transmitted in multiparty systems with weak party identification and high electoral volatility. We propose a model of intergenerational transmission that includes both direct effects of parents' party preferences on those of their children, as well as indirect effects through left–right and issue positions. We test this model with original survey data of Dutch adolescents (14–20 years old) and their parents (N = 751 adolescent-parent pairs). We find two paths through which parents exert influence on the party preferences of their adolescent children. On the first path, parental party preferences function as a direct predictor of adolescent party preferences. On the second path, adolescent left–right and issue positions function as a mediator between parental left–right and issue positions and adolescent party preferences, with the effect of left–right positions being stronger than that of issue positions. The frequency with which adolescents discuss political topics with their parents moderates these effects. 相似文献
59.
60.
Research on self-control has flourished within the last two decades, with many researchers trying to answer one of the most fundamental questions regarding human behaviour—how do we successfully regulate desires in the pursuit of long-term goals? While recent research has focused on different strategies to enhance self-control success, we still know very little about how strategies are implemented or where the need for self-control comes from in the first place. Drawing from parallel fields (e.g., emotion regulation, health) and other theories of self-regulation, we propose an integrative framework that describes self-control as a dynamic, multi-stage process that unfolds over time. In this review, we first provide an overview of this framework, which poses three stages of regulation: the identification of the need for self-control, the selection of strategies to regulate temptations, and the implementation of chosen strategies. These regulatory stages are then flexibly monitored over time. We then expand this framework by outlining a series of growth points to guide future research. By bridging across theories and disciplines, the present framework improves our understanding of how self-control unfolds in everyday life. 相似文献