全文获取类型
收费全文 | 522篇 |
免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
527篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 4篇 |
2019年 | 9篇 |
2018年 | 8篇 |
2017年 | 10篇 |
2016年 | 10篇 |
2015年 | 11篇 |
2014年 | 11篇 |
2013年 | 63篇 |
2012年 | 19篇 |
2011年 | 16篇 |
2010年 | 5篇 |
2009年 | 11篇 |
2008年 | 12篇 |
2007年 | 22篇 |
2006年 | 16篇 |
2005年 | 14篇 |
2004年 | 11篇 |
2003年 | 13篇 |
2002年 | 17篇 |
2001年 | 9篇 |
2000年 | 8篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 12篇 |
1996年 | 4篇 |
1995年 | 10篇 |
1994年 | 6篇 |
1993年 | 10篇 |
1992年 | 7篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1984年 | 6篇 |
1983年 | 5篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 11篇 |
1973年 | 3篇 |
1972年 | 6篇 |
1971年 | 8篇 |
1969年 | 9篇 |
1968年 | 7篇 |
1967年 | 8篇 |
1966年 | 3篇 |
1964年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有527条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
431.
Nicholas L. Vasilopoulos Jeffrey M. Cucina Theodore L. Hayes 《International Journal of Selection & Assessment》2005,13(1):92-95
This study investigated whether scores of item response accuracy (AC) and item response latencies (RLs) from a computer based artificial language test (ALT) uniquely predicted grades in a Spanish language course at a U.S. law enforcement training academy. As expected, ALT–RLs added to the prediction of course grades provided by ALT–AC. This finding held even after controlling for scores on a logical reasoning test. Results are discussed in terms of the potential benefits of using RLs to predict performance in training. 相似文献
432.
Theodore R. Curry 《Deviant behavior》2013,34(6):571-599
ABSTRACT A major innovation in control balance theory is the integration of motivating and constraining forces into a “causal chain” explanation of deviance. To test these relationships, this research presents a parsed model of the causal chain that focuses on the associations among control ratios, deviant motivation, situational provocations, constraint, and deviant behavior. Results from a first-person scenario testing hypotheses from this model show that, as predicted, control ratios affect deviance both directly and indirectly through motivating and constraining variables. While the full model explains about 39% of the variation in deviance, control ratios do not, however, show great explanatory power regarding variation in motivation and constraint. 相似文献
433.
Theodore Korzukhin 《Thought: A Journal of Philosophy》2014,3(3):177-183
Lewis (1976) argued that ‘there is no way to interpret a conditional connective so that, with sufficient generality, the probabilities of conditionals will equal the appropriate conditional probabilities’. However, as Lewis and others have subsequently recognized, Lewis' triviality results go through only on the assumption that ‘if’ is not context‐sensitive. This leaves a question that has not been adequately addressed: what are the prospects of a context‐sensitive theory of ‘if’ that complies with Stalnaker's thesis? I offer one interesting constraint on any such theory. I argue that no context‐sensitive theory satisfies Stalnaker's thesis if it satisfies three plausible assumptions: first, that the truth of an indicative is determined by the world of evaluation and by the set of worlds in the relevant epistemic context in which the antecedent is true; second, that one can learn an indicative conditional without learning that the antecedent and consequent are both true; third, that belief revision is conservative in the sense that it does not reduce the probabilities to zero unnecessarily. The result gives us a clearer picture of the real costs of a truth‐conditional context‐sensitive Stalnaker's thesis‐compliant semantics. 相似文献
434.
Fixed-ratio (FR) size was increased for pigeons (N = 6), while the number of eating responses at reinforcement was either held constant or increased. Also, FR size was held constant while the number of eating responses at reinforcement was increased. Major findings were as follows: (a) when eating responses were held constant, higher FR requirements led to stable key rates and postreinforcement pauses; (b) allowing more eating responses at successively higher FR requirements led to stable key rates and postreinforcement pauses; (c) at high FR requirements which produced low key rates, increasing the number of eating responses at reinforcement increased key rates; and (d) eating response rates varied systematically with key rates. 相似文献
435.
Dr. Theodore L. Dorpat M.D. 《Psychoanalytic Inquiry》2013,33(3):448-463
An unorthodox view of the function of primary process is presented with a view to enlarge, rather than diminish Freudian assumptions. One of the basic tenets of Freudian hypothesis was the supposed lack of influence of outside experience upon primary process functions. Yet we see demonstrated on a daily basis that primary process cognition is at work in any human interaction or experience. The mental structures of the self in interaction with the nonself is constantly monitored, added to, or subtracted from during contact with others. It is a prereflective mode that does not immediately rise into awareness. We give meaning to all interactions without necessarily reflecting upon them or even clarifying them to ourselves. Without allowing for such meaning to be integrated, we would lose or misinterpret large portions of our daily interactions. Flaws in communication occur every day and are demonstrable particularly in therapy when therapists are not attuned to their patients' emotional needs. It is demonstrated that nonverbal avoidance behavior of disturbed infants is the precursor for disturbed object relations of adults. Therefore, it makes no sense to interpret unconscious meanings—there are none. The difficulty lies in the inter-subjective realm and serves defensive modes. However, if patients present an inauthentic self, it is often difficult to decide if one is indeed in the presence of such a maimed self. The therapist's countertransference aids in detecting inaulhenticity. Ever-present, unconscious meaning analysis must be brought into consciousness by the therapist in order to further the therapeutic process. 相似文献
436.
Ethan R. Van Norman Peter M. Nelson Jae-Eun Shin Theodore J. Christ 《Journal of Behavioral Education》2013,22(4):283-301
Educators, school psychologists, and other professionals must evaluate student progress and decide to continue, modify, or terminate instructional programs to ensure student success. For this purpose, progress-monitoring data are often collected, plotted graphically, and visually analyzed. The current study evaluated the impact of three common formats for visual analysis (scatter plots, scatter plots with trend lines, and scatter plots with trend and aim lines) on the decision accuracy of 52 novice analysts. All participants viewed 18 time-series graphs that depicted student growth on a continuous metric (e.g., oral reading fluency). Participants rated each graph as depicting substantial progress, minimal progress, or no progress. The magnitude of the true slope for each graph was fixed to 3.00 (substantial progress), 0.75 (minimal progress), or 0 (no progress). Inferential analyses were used to determine the probability of a participant correctly identifying different magnitudes of trend in the presence of different visual aids (no visual aid, trend line, and trend line with aim line). The odds of correctly identifying trend were influenced by visual aid (p < .01) and trend magnitude (p < .01). The addition of a trend line resulted in a sharp increase in the probability of making a correct decision. Graphs depicting minimal progress reduced the probability of a correct decision. 相似文献
437.
438.
439.
440.