全文获取类型
收费全文 | 577篇 |
免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
584篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 5篇 |
2019年 | 11篇 |
2018年 | 13篇 |
2017年 | 11篇 |
2016年 | 12篇 |
2015年 | 12篇 |
2014年 | 13篇 |
2013年 | 77篇 |
2012年 | 22篇 |
2011年 | 16篇 |
2010年 | 6篇 |
2009年 | 12篇 |
2008年 | 13篇 |
2007年 | 25篇 |
2006年 | 17篇 |
2005年 | 16篇 |
2004年 | 11篇 |
2003年 | 14篇 |
2002年 | 17篇 |
2001年 | 10篇 |
2000年 | 10篇 |
1999年 | 8篇 |
1997年 | 12篇 |
1996年 | 4篇 |
1995年 | 10篇 |
1994年 | 6篇 |
1993年 | 10篇 |
1992年 | 7篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 6篇 |
1988年 | 5篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 8篇 |
1984年 | 6篇 |
1983年 | 5篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 12篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 12篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
1972年 | 6篇 |
1971年 | 8篇 |
1969年 | 9篇 |
1968年 | 8篇 |
1967年 | 9篇 |
1964年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有584条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
441.
442.
This study used a community sample of 192 intact families to examine two issues related to conceptualization and measurement of family functioning. First, a confirmatory factor model supported the adequacy of using dyadic-level self-report measures (e.g., mother-child, child-father) to identify three family level constructs of Family Affect, Family Control, and Family Shared Activity. Second, data derived from behavioral observations during dyadic interactions identified two factors, Family Positivity and Family Negativity. Correlations among the self-report and behavioral observation factors indicated high Family Positivity was significantly associated with higher Family Affect and lower Family Control, and Family Negativity was significantly associated with lower Family Affect and with lower Family Shared Activity. Findings are discussed in terms of limitations and directions for research. 相似文献
443.
Shapiro T 《Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association》2007,55(2):469-78; discussion 493-501
444.
445.
446.
Evans TA 《Learning and motivation》2007,38(4):342-357
The variables of delay and effort have been found to influence self-control predictably and in similar fashion when tested independently, but it is unclear how they influence self-control interactively. In the present study, I tested these two variables simultaneously to gain better understanding of their combined influence on self-control. A computerized task was employed in which monkey participants could sequence 1 or more digital images before “cashing-out,” after which they would receive their accumulated rewards. Delay was manipulated by adjusting the speed of the cursor used to select images. Cognitive effort was manipulated by presenting image sets that appeared in either a constant or a randomized configuration. For most monkeys, an interaction was found between the effects of delay and effort on the number of images selected before cashing-out. The results suggest that, when combined, these two variables have a complex influence on self-control. 相似文献
447.
In this paper I provide some formal schemas for theanalysis of vague predicatesin terms of a set of semantic relations other thanclassical synonymy, includingweak synonymy (as between ``large' and ``huge'),antonymy (as between ``large'and ``small'), relativity (as between ``large'and ``large for a dog'), and a kind ofsupervenience (as between ``large' and ``wide'or ``long'). All of these relationsare representable in the simple comparativelogic CL, in accordance with the basicformula: the more something is F, the more(or less) it is G. I use Carnapianmeaning postulates to define these relationsas constraints on interpretations of theformal language of CL. 相似文献
448.
Theodore Sider 《Philosophical Studies》2017,174(10):2467-2478
Elizabeth Barnes and Mari Mikkola raise the important question of whether certain recent approaches to metaphysics exclude feminist metaphysics. My own approach (from my book Writing the Book of the World) does not, or so I argue. I do define “substantive” questions in terms of fundamentality; and the concepts of feminist metaphysics (and social metaphysics generally) are nonfundamental. But my definition does not count a question as being nonsubstantive simply because it involves nonfundamental concepts. Questions about the causal structure of the world, including the causal structure of the social world, are generally substantive because their answers are not sensitive to any alternate, equally good conceptual choices we could have made. I also argue that such questions are substantive regardless of the ontology of social kinds. 相似文献
449.
Shapiro T 《Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association》2008,56(1):123-146
During adolescence the central masturbation fantasy that is formulated during childhood takes its final form and paradoxically must now be directed outward for appropriate object finding and pair matching in the service of procreative aims. This is a step in adaptation that requires a further developmental landmark that I have called normalization. The path toward airing these private fantasies is facilitated by chumship relationships as a step toward further exposure to the social surround. Hartmann's structuring application of adaptation within psychoanalysis is used as a framework for understanding the process that simultaneously serves intrapsychic and social demands and permits goals that follow evolutionary principles. Variations in the normalization process from masturbatory isolation to a variety of forms of sexual socialization are examined in sociological data concerning current adolescent sexual behavior and in case examples that indicate some routes to normalized experience and practice. 相似文献
450.