排序方式: 共有46条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
11.
12.
Yuichiro Kikuno Tetsuro Matsunaga Jun Saiki 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》2013,75(7):1427-1437
The CHRNA4 gene is known to be associated with individual differences in attention. However, its associations with other cognitive functions remain to be elucidated. In the present study, we investigated the effects of genetic variations in CHRNA4 on rapid scene categorization by 100 healthy human participants. In Experiment 1, we also conducted the Attention Network Test (ANT) in order to examine whether the genetic effects could be accounted for by attention. CHRNA4 was genotyped as carrying the TT, CT, or CC allele. The scene categorization task required participants to judge whether the category of a scene image (natural or man-made) was consistent with a cue word displayed at the response phase. The target–mask stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) ranged from 13 to 93 ms. In comparison with CC-allele carriers, CT- and TT-allele carriers responded more accurately at the long SOA (93 ms) only during natural-scene categorization. In contrast, we observed no consistent association between CHRNA4 and the ANT, and no intertask correlation between scene categorization and the ANT. To validate our natural-scene categorization results, Experiment 2, carried out with an independent sample of 100 participants and a different stimulus set, successfully replicated the association between CHRNA4 genotypes and natural-scene categorization accuracy at long SOAs (67 and 93 ms). Our findings demonstrate, for the first time, that genetic variations in CHRNA4 can moderately contribute to individual differences in natural-scene categorization performance. 相似文献
13.
A new form of taste aversion conditioning was established in the pond snail Lymnaea stagnalis. An associative memory, lasting 24h, was produced in the pond snail with 20 pairings of 100 mM sucrose as the conditioned stimulus (CS) and mechanical stimulation to the head as the unconditioned stimulus (UCS). Animals exposed to reverse pairings of the CS and UCS failed to learn the association. The learning was characterized by a shift in the response to the UCS from a whole-body withdrawal response to the cessation of feeding behavior. 相似文献
14.
We investigated the behavioural and cognitive development of a captive male infant chimpanzee, Ayumu, raised by his mother, Ai. Here we report Ayumu's achievements up to the age of 2 years and 3 months, in the context of complex computer-controlled tasks. From soon after birth, Ayumu had been present during an experiment performed by his mother. The task consisted of two phases, a matching-to-sample task in which she received token rewards, and the insertion of these tokens into a vending machine to obtain food rewards. Ayumu himself received no reward or encouragement from humans for any of the actions he exhibited during the experiment. At the age of 9 months and 3 weeks, Ayumu performed his first matching-to-sample trial. At around 1 year and 3 months, he began to perform them consistently. Also during this period, he frequently stole food rewards from his mother. At 2 years and 3 months, Ayumu succeeded for the first time in inserting a token into the vending machine. Once he had succeeded in using a token, he performed both phases of the task in sequence 20 times consecutively. The infant's behaviour was not shaped by food rewards but by a strong motivation to copy his mother's behaviour. Our observations of Ayumu thus mirror the learning processes shown by wild chimpanzees. 相似文献
15.
This study focuses on the development of spontaneous object manipulation in three infant chimpanzees during their first 2 years of life. The three infants were raised by their biological mothers who lived among a group of chimpanzees. A human tester conducted a series of cognitive tests in a triadic situation where mothers collaborated with the researcher during the testing of the infants. Four tasks were presented, taken from normative studies of cognitive development of Japanese infants: inserting objects into corresponding holes in a box, seriating nesting cups, inserting variously shaped objects into corresponding holes in a template, and stacking up wooden blocks. The mothers had already acquired skills to perform these manipulation tasks. The infants were free to observe the mothers' manipulative behavior from immediately after birth. We focused on object–object combinations that were made spontaneously by the infant chimpanzees, without providing food reinforcement for any specific behavior that the infants performed. The three main findings can be summarized as follows. First, there was precocious appearance of object–object combination in infant chimpanzees: the age of onset (8–11 months) was comparable to that in humans (around 10 months old).Second, object–object combinations in chimpanzees remained at a low frequency between 11 and 16 months, then increased dramatically at the age of approximately 1.5 years. At the same time, the accuracy of these object–object combinations also increased. Third, chimpanzee infants showed inserting behavior frequently and from an early age but they did not exhibit stacking behavior during their first 2 years of life, in clear contrast to human data. 相似文献
16.
17.
The use of gaze shifts as social cues has various evolutionary advantages. To investigate the developmental processes of this ability, we conducted an object-choice task by using longitudinal methods with infant chimpanzees tested from 8 months old until 3 years old. The experimenter used one of six gestures towards a cup concealing food; tapping, touching, whole-hand pointing, gazing plus close-pointing, distant-pointing, close-gazing, and distant-gazing. Unlike any other previous study, we analyzed the behavioral changes that occurred before and after choosing the cup. We assumed that pre-choice behavior indicates the development of an attentional and spatial connection between a pointing cue and an object (e.g. Woodward, 2005); and post-choice behavior indicates the emergence of object permanence (e.g. Piaget, 1954). Our study demonstrated that infant chimpanzees begin to use experimenter-given cues with age (after 11 months of age). Moreover, the results from the behavioral analysis showed that the infants gradually developed the spatial link between the pointing as an object-directed action and the object. Moreover, when they were 11 months old, the infants began to inspect the inside of the cup, suggesting the onset of object permanence. Overall, our results imply that the ability to use the cues is developing and mutually related with other cognitive developments. The present study also suggests what the standard object-choice task actually measures by breaking the task down into the developmental trajectories of its component parts, and describes for the first time the social-physical cognitive development during the task with a longitudinal method. 相似文献
18.
An adult female chimpanzee with previous training in the use of Arabic numerals 1–9 was introduced to the meaning of "zero"
in the context of three different numerical tasks. The first two were cardinal tasks where the subject was required either
to select numerals corresponding to the number of items presented on a computer screen (productive use of numerals) or to
match sets of the appropriate size to numerals presented as samples (receptive use). The third task addressed the ordinal
meaning of the same symbols where the subject was required to respond to numerals sequentially, arranging them into an ascending
series. The subject mastered the recognition of the meaning of zero in all three tasks. However, details of her usage of the
symbol revealed that transfer of the meaning between different kinds of tasks was incomplete, suggesting that the level of
abstraction characteristic of human numerical ability was not attained in the chimpanzee. Over the course of acquisition leading
to the high levels of accuracy eventually observed, the newly introduced zero appeared to shift along the length of a continuous
numerical scale toward the lower end, while confusions with 1 remained the most frequently encountered mistakes. Such patterns
of error thus suggest that Ai's understanding of the meaning of zero in relation to the rest of the number symbols was not
consistent with an "absence of items versus presence of items" scheme.
Electronic Publication 相似文献
19.
These experiments investigated how chimpanzees learn to navigate visual fingermazes presented on a touch monitor. The aim
was to determine whether training the subjects to solve several different mazes would establish a generalized map-reading
skill such that they would solve new mazes correctly on the first presentation. In experiment 1, two captive adult female
chimpanzees were trained to move a visual object (a ball) with a finger over the monitor surface toward a target through a
grid of obstacles that formed a maze. The task was fully automated with storage of movement paths on individual trials. Training
progressed from very simple mazes with one obstacle to complex mazes with several obstacles. The subjects learned to move
the ball to the target in a curved path so as to avoid obstacles and blind alleys. After training on several mazes, both subjects
developed a high level of efficiency in moving the ball to the target in a path that closely approached the ideal shortest
path. New mazes were then presented to determine whether the subjects had acquired a more generalized maze-solving performance.
The subjects solved 65–100% of the new mazes the first time they were presented by moving the ball around obstacles to the
target without making detours into blind alleys. In experiment 2, one of the chimpanzees was trained using mazes with two
routes to the target. One of the routes was blocked at one of many possible locations. After training to avoid the blind alley
in different mazes, new mazes were presented that also had one route blocked. The subject correctly solved 90.7% of the novel
mazes. When the mazes had one short and one long open route to the target the subject preferred the shorter route. When the
short route was blocked, the subject solved only 53.3% of the mazes because of the preference for the shorter route even when
blocked. The overall results suggest that with the training methods used the subjects learned to solve specific mazes with
a trial-and-error method. Although both subjects were able to solve many of the novel mazes they did not fully develop a more
general "map-reading" skill.
Accepted after revision: 30 July 2001
Electronic Publication 相似文献
20.
One advantage of living in a social group is the opportunity to use information provided by other individuals. Social information can be based on cues provided by a conspecific or even by a heterospecific individual (e.g., gaze direction, vocalizations, pointing gestures). Although the use of human gaze and gestures has been extensively studied in primates, and is increasingly studied in other mammals, there is no documentation of birds using these cues in a cooperative context. In this study, we tested the ability of three African gray parrots to use different human cues (pointing and/or gazing) in an object-choice task. We found that one subject spontaneously used the most salient pointing gesture (looking and steady pointing with hand at about 20 cm from the baited box). The two others were also able to use this cue after 15 trials. None of the parrots spontaneously used the steady gaze cues (combined head and eye orientation), but one learned to do so effectively after only 15 trials when the distance between the head and the baited box was about 1 m. However, none of the parrots were able to use the momentary pointing nor the distal pointing and gazing cues. These results are discussed in terms of sensitivity to joint attention as a prerequisite to understand pointing gestures as it is to the referential use of labels. 相似文献