全文获取类型
收费全文 | 928篇 |
免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
939篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 8篇 |
2023年 | 9篇 |
2021年 | 8篇 |
2020年 | 8篇 |
2019年 | 14篇 |
2018年 | 19篇 |
2017年 | 18篇 |
2016年 | 14篇 |
2015年 | 15篇 |
2014年 | 22篇 |
2013年 | 93篇 |
2012年 | 33篇 |
2011年 | 28篇 |
2010年 | 26篇 |
2009年 | 24篇 |
2008年 | 26篇 |
2007年 | 36篇 |
2006年 | 33篇 |
2005年 | 30篇 |
2004年 | 38篇 |
2003年 | 36篇 |
2002年 | 31篇 |
2001年 | 15篇 |
2000年 | 19篇 |
1999年 | 19篇 |
1998年 | 12篇 |
1997年 | 11篇 |
1996年 | 14篇 |
1995年 | 13篇 |
1994年 | 18篇 |
1993年 | 10篇 |
1992年 | 19篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1989年 | 12篇 |
1988年 | 11篇 |
1987年 | 7篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 10篇 |
1984年 | 10篇 |
1983年 | 13篇 |
1982年 | 14篇 |
1981年 | 15篇 |
1980年 | 16篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1977年 | 13篇 |
1976年 | 8篇 |
1975年 | 11篇 |
1974年 | 9篇 |
1971年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有939条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
Online comprehension of naturally spoken and perceptually degraded words was assessed in 95 children ages 12 to 31 months. The time course of word recognition was measured by monitoring eye movements as children looked at pictures while listening to familiar target words presented in unaltered, time-compressed, and low-pass-filtered forms. Success in word recognition varied with age and level of vocabulary development, and with the perceptual integrity of the word. Recognition was best overall for unaltered words, lower for time-compressed words, and significantly lower in low-pass-filtered words. Reaction times were fastest in compressed, followed by unaltered and filtered words. Results showed that children were able to recognize familiar words in challenging conditions and that productive vocabulary size was more sensitive than chronological age as a predictor of children's accuracy and speed in word recognition. 相似文献
62.
63.
64.
65.
W. Trammell Neill Kathleen M. Terry Leslie A. Valdes 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》1994,1(1):119-121
Responses to an object may be slower or less accurate if that object shares attributes with a recently ignored object(negative priming). Some studies have found negative priming only if the probe trial required selection against a distractor stimulus. In the present experiment, subjects responded to the location of a target (O), ignoring a distractor (X) if it appeared in another location. Reaction time was slower to probe targets that appeared in the same location as the prime distractor, regardless of whether or not the probe target was accompanied by a distractor. 相似文献
66.
Support for lateralization of the Whorf effect beyond the realm of color discrimination 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Recent work has shown that Whorf effects of language on color discrimination are stronger in the right visual field than in the left. Here we show that this phenomenon is not limited to color: The perception of animal figures (cats and dogs) was more strongly affected by linguistic categories for stimuli presented to the right visual field than those presented to the left. Moreover, the magnitude of the visual field asymmetry was reduced when demands on verbal working memory were increased by a secondary task. This reduction did not occur when the secondary task imposed demands on spatial working memory. Taken together, these results demonstrate that the lateralized Whorf effect may be quite general, reflecting an interaction of linguistic and perceptual codes primarily in the left hemisphere. 相似文献
67.
Megela Simmons A Simmons JA Bates ME 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》2008,122(3):274-282
Analysis of acoustic interactions between animals in active choruses is complex because of the large numbers of individuals present, their high calling rates, and the considerable numbers of vocalizations that either overlap or show close temporal alternation. The authors describe a methodology for recording chorus activity in bullfrogs (Rana catesbeiana) using multiple, closely spaced acoustic sensors that provide simultaneous estimates of sound direction and sound characteristics. This method provides estimates of location of individual callers, even under conditions of call overlap. This is a useful technique for understanding the complexity of the acoustic scene faced by animals vocalizing in groups. 相似文献
68.
Moral phenomenology is (roughly) the study of those features of occurrent mental states with moral significance which are
accessible through direct introspection, whether or not such states possess phenomenal character – a what-it-is-likeness.
In this paper, as the title indicates, we introduce and make prefatory remarks about moral phenomenology and its significance
for ethics. After providing a brief taxonomy of types of moral experience, we proceed to consider questions about the commonality within and distinctiveness of such experiences, with an eye on some of the main philosophical issues in ethics and how moral phenomenology might be brought
to bear on them. In discussing such matters, we consider some of the doubts about moral phenomenology and its value to ethics
that are brought up by Walter Sinnott-Armstrong and Michael Gill in their contributions to this issue.
相似文献
Mark Timmons (Corresponding author)Email: |
69.
Abstract The present study was designed to examine the utility of the theory of reasoned action (Fishbein and Azjen, 1975) in relation to people's propensity to engage in a range of different safe sex strategies, including engaging in an exclusive sexual relationship, avoiding casual sex, and asking sexual partners about their previous sexual and IV drug use history. It was proposed that the intention to engage in a particular safe sex practice would be influenced by the person's attitude towards engaging in the practice, as well as his or her perception of the extent to which others thought they should do so (subjective norm), while behavioural intentions were proposed to predict actual behaviour. The study was also designed to examine whether, after control of the effects of the components of the theory of reasoned action, the person's generalised control beliefs would explain any additional variance in either behavioural intentions or actual behaviour. One hundred and two sexually active heterosexual students participated in the study. At the first wave of data collection, subjects completed measures of intentions, attitudes, and norms appropriate to each of the different behaviours. Measures of actual behaviour were obtained at follow-tip (three months after first period of data collection). The results of the study indicated that attitudes and norms predicted intentions for avoiding casual sex and asking sexual partners about their previous sexual and IV drug use history, while intentions predicted actual behaviour for all three safe sex strategies. Although there were no significant main effects of control beliefs on intentions or behaviour, subjects with internal control beliefs were more likely than their counterparts with external control beliefs to behave in accordance with their intentions to engage in an exclusive sexual relationship and ask sexual partners about their previous sexual and IV drug use history. Additional analyses revealed that the levels and determinants of intentions and actual behaviour were, in general, similar for males and females. 相似文献
70.
F Dick E Bates B Wulfeck J A Utman N Dronkers M A Gernsbacher 《Psychological review》2001,108(4):759-788
Selective deficits in aphasic patients' grammatical production and comprehension are often cited as evidence that syntactic processing is modular and localizable in discrete areas of the brain (e.g., Y. Grodzinsky, 2000). The authors review a large body of experimental evidence suggesting that morpho-syntactic deficits can be observed in a number of aphasic and neurologically intact populations. They present new data showing that receptive agrammatism is found not only over a range of aphasic groups, but is also observed in neurologically intact individuals processing under stressful conditions. The authors suggest that these data are most compatible with a domain-general account of language, one that emphasizes the interaction of linguistic distributions with the properties of an associative processor working under normal or suboptimal conditions. 相似文献