全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1422篇 |
免费 | 45篇 |
专业分类
1467篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 15篇 |
2020年 | 20篇 |
2019年 | 22篇 |
2018年 | 30篇 |
2017年 | 49篇 |
2016年 | 36篇 |
2015年 | 27篇 |
2014年 | 34篇 |
2013年 | 127篇 |
2012年 | 53篇 |
2011年 | 55篇 |
2010年 | 43篇 |
2009年 | 51篇 |
2008年 | 55篇 |
2007年 | 49篇 |
2006年 | 45篇 |
2005年 | 40篇 |
2004年 | 44篇 |
2003年 | 34篇 |
2002年 | 33篇 |
2001年 | 36篇 |
2000年 | 53篇 |
1999年 | 41篇 |
1998年 | 24篇 |
1997年 | 20篇 |
1996年 | 18篇 |
1995年 | 16篇 |
1994年 | 15篇 |
1993年 | 14篇 |
1992年 | 16篇 |
1991年 | 20篇 |
1990年 | 17篇 |
1989年 | 17篇 |
1988年 | 11篇 |
1987年 | 20篇 |
1985年 | 16篇 |
1984年 | 13篇 |
1983年 | 11篇 |
1981年 | 10篇 |
1980年 | 12篇 |
1979年 | 12篇 |
1977年 | 11篇 |
1975年 | 9篇 |
1974年 | 16篇 |
1973年 | 9篇 |
1971年 | 13篇 |
1968年 | 11篇 |
1967年 | 10篇 |
1966年 | 17篇 |
1965年 | 14篇 |
排序方式: 共有1467条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
82.
83.
Paul Thompson 《欧洲心理治疗、咨询与健康杂志》2013,15(1):71-85
This paper offers a psychoanalytic exploration of meaning within the concept of ecology. From a starting point in the experience of primal anxiety, originating in the work of Freud, Jung and Klein, links are made to cultural themes through a developing comparison of case studies in literature, mythology, the public sphere and clinical practice. It is argued that the application of psychoanalytic ideas to environmental concern has validity. The idea emerges that individual and public attitudes to our environment might be open to positive change through an appreciation of the concept of early anxiety, currently available to us in work of the imagination. 相似文献
84.
85.
In the present study, we investigated how well Lee's (1973/1976) typology of love characterized response data that adult subjects provided on selected items written by Hendrick and Hendrick. Results suggest that the model represents response data reasonably well and that Hendrick and Hendrick's (1986) measure ought to be valid. 相似文献
86.
Mark A. Barnett Mark A. Thompson Jane Schroff 《The Journal of genetic psychology》2013,174(4):489-498
Second-grade, fourth-grade, sixth-grade, high school, and college students (N = 140) were asked to describe situations in which they would not help someone else and their reasons for not helping in those situations. Younger subjects used perceived self-incompetence as a reason for not helping more frequently than did older subjects. In contrast, older subjects more frequently cited a desire not to violate another's personal situation and/or their own internalized values, laws, and rules as reasons not to help than did their younger counterparts. For a majority of the not-helping categories, however, no significant effect of age level was found, and indeed, some individuals in even the youngest age group expressed subtle and insightful reasons for not helping. 相似文献
87.
Although linguistic traditions of the last century assumed that there is no link between sound and meaning (i.e., arbitrariness), recent research has established a nonarbitrary relation between sound and meaning (i.e., sound symbolism). For example, some sounds (e.g., /u/ as in took) suggest bigness whereas others (e.g., /i/ as in tiny) suggest smallness. We tested whether sound symbolism only marks contrasts (e.g., small versus big things) or whether it marks object properties in a graded manner (e.g., small, medium, and large things). In two experiments, participants viewed novel objects (i.e., greebles) of varying size and chose the most appropriate name for each object from a list of visually or auditorily presented nonwords that varied incrementally in the number of "large" and "small" phonemes. For instance, "wodolo" contains all large-sounding phonemes, whereas "kitete" contains all small-sounding phonemes. Participants' choices revealed a graded relationship between sound and size: The size of the object linearly predicted the number of large-sounding phonemes in its preferred name. That is, small, medium, and large objects elicited names with increasing numbers of large-sounding phonemes. The results are discussed in relation to cross-modal processing, gesture, and vocal pitch. 相似文献
88.
The current study was designed to determine whether the inclusion of a disclaimer (i.e., “Retouched photograph aimed at changing a person's physical appearance.”) or warning (i.e., “Warning: Trying to look as thin as this model may be dangerous to your health.”) added to images of thin/attractive models would affect body dissatisfaction and intent to diet in female undergraduate students (n = 342). Participants were randomly assigned to one of four groups: (a) disclaimer, (b) warning, (c) model control, or (d) car control. Results revealed a significant interaction between group and time, whereby only the car control group reported a significant change (i.e., decrease) in body dissatisfaction over time. Groups did not differ on intent to diet measured at post-exposure. The results largely replicate other findings in this area and call into question advocacy efforts to label media images as a strategy to decrease women's identification with the stimuli. 相似文献
89.
Charles E. Naquin Terri R. Kurtzberg 《Organizational behavior and human decision processes》2004,93(2):129
Two experiments examine how the identification of technology as a causal factor in an organizational accident influences judgments of organizational accountability. In study 1, organizations were found to be held less accountable for their actions when a misfortune was rooted in a computer error than when rooted in human error. The predicted mechanism for this effect, counterfactual thinking, was confirmed. Specifically, technologically induced accidents were found to generate fewer counterfactual thoughts of better possible outcomes than similar accidents resulting from human error. Study 2 replicated the findings of study 1 in a more natural setting and using a less intrusive measure for counterfactual thoughts. 相似文献
90.
Brown AL Messman-Moore TL Miller AG Stasser G 《Personality & social psychology bulletin》2005,31(7):963-976
There is evidence that personal experience with trauma is associated with increases in both personal and comparative risk perception. This study investigates this relation in terms of sexual victimization among women, focusing on three potential mediators: perceived control over sexual assault, perceived similarity to a typical sexual assault victim, and psychological distress. Mediational analyses were investigated using structural equation modeling. Although victimization experience was not related to comparative risk perception, it was associated with greater personal risk perception. This relation was mediated by perceived similarity to a typical sexual assault victim. Prospective analyses indicated that personal risk perception does change in response to sexual victimization but also indicated that heightened risk perception may be an accurate assessment of risk that actually precedes victimization experience. Implications for the meaning of perceived similarity and perceptions of risk for sexual assault victims are discussed. 相似文献