首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   17972篇
  免费   857篇
  国内免费   15篇
  2021年   146篇
  2020年   247篇
  2019年   268篇
  2018年   400篇
  2017年   396篇
  2016年   406篇
  2015年   312篇
  2014年   333篇
  2013年   1414篇
  2012年   616篇
  2011年   706篇
  2010年   417篇
  2009年   368篇
  2008年   523篇
  2007年   500篇
  2006年   439篇
  2005年   368篇
  2004年   372篇
  2003年   351篇
  2002年   361篇
  2001年   718篇
  2000年   724篇
  1999年   478篇
  1998年   198篇
  1997年   186篇
  1996年   155篇
  1995年   144篇
  1993年   152篇
  1992年   372篇
  1991年   334篇
  1990年   357篇
  1989年   343篇
  1988年   327篇
  1987年   297篇
  1986年   323篇
  1985年   311篇
  1984年   244篇
  1983年   210篇
  1979年   193篇
  1978年   168篇
  1975年   190篇
  1974年   245篇
  1973年   202篇
  1972年   208篇
  1971年   169篇
  1970年   152篇
  1969年   199篇
  1968年   205篇
  1967年   161篇
  1966年   184篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
261.
262.
The reading performance of a Japanese Broca-type aphasic patient on a single-word reading test was investigated. The result indicated that the subject fits the symptom complex of deep dyslexia in more than one aspect. Unique characteristics of this subject included (1) the isolated subcortical site of the lesion, which apparently produced deep dyslexia, and (2) double dissociations between kanji and kana processing and between oral reading and reading comprehension. The performance of this subject was compared with that of another Japanese deep dyslexic subject in S. Sasanuma (1980, In M. Coltheart, K. Patterson, & J. C. Marshall (Eds.), Deep dyslexia, London: Routledge & Kegan Paul). A theoretical implication was attempted based on a dual coding process scheme by S. Sasanuma and O. Fujimura (1978, Cortex, 7, 1-18).  相似文献   
263.
264.
265.
R K Mills 《Adolescence》1987,22(87):641-646
This article explores the use of fiction in the classroom as a tool for enhancing the self-understanding of adolescents. Specifically, two novels by S.E. Hinton are used as examples. Theoretical consideration as well as practical classroom activities are discussed.  相似文献   
266.
Dittmar, Warm, and Dember (1985) suggested that visual parallax may lead to declines in perceptual sensitivity over time in spatial vigilance tasks involving comparative judgments. The present study tested this possibility by restraining subjects' head movements during a 1-hr vigil in which comparative (simultaneous task) or absolute (successive task) judgments of line length were necessary for signal detection. Under free-viewing conditions, perceptual sensitivity declined over time with both types of tasks. Head restraint eliminated the sensitivity decrement in both cases. The results highlight signal quality as a crucial determinant of perceptual decrements in sustained attention.  相似文献   
267.
Memory bias in clinical anxiety   总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6  
  相似文献   
268.
269.
270.
We examined social-information-processing mechanisms (e.g., hostile attributional biases and intention-cue detection deficits) in chronic reactive and proactive aggressive behavior in children's peer groups. In Study 1, a teacher-rating instrument was developed to assess these behaviors in elementary school children (N = 259). Reactive and proactive scales were found to be internally consistent, and factor analyses partially supported convergent and discriminant validities. In Study 2, behavioral correlates of these forms of aggression were examined through assessments by peers (N = 339). Both types of aggression related to social rejection, but only proactively aggressive boys were also viewed as leaders and as having a sense of humor. In Study 3, we hypothesized that reactive aggression (but not proactive aggression) would occur as a function of hostile attributional biases and intention-cue detection deficits. Four groups of socially rejected boys (reactive aggressive, proactive aggressive, reactive-proactive aggressive, and nonaggressive) and a group of average boys were presented with a series of hypothetical videorecorded vignettes depicting provocations by peers and were asked to interpret the intentions of the provocateur (N = 117). Only the two reactive-aggressive groups displayed biases and deficits in interpretations. In Study 4, attributional biases and deficits were found to be positively correlated with the rate of reactive aggression (but not proactive aggression) displayed in free play with peers (N = 127). These studies supported the hypothesis that attributional biases and deficits are related to reactive aggression but not to proactive aggression.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号