全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2106篇 |
免费 | 21篇 |
专业分类
2127篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 15篇 |
2023年 | 17篇 |
2022年 | 13篇 |
2021年 | 28篇 |
2020年 | 46篇 |
2019年 | 35篇 |
2018年 | 66篇 |
2017年 | 69篇 |
2016年 | 62篇 |
2015年 | 47篇 |
2014年 | 60篇 |
2013年 | 232篇 |
2012年 | 119篇 |
2011年 | 94篇 |
2010年 | 66篇 |
2009年 | 59篇 |
2008年 | 73篇 |
2007年 | 83篇 |
2006年 | 61篇 |
2005年 | 74篇 |
2004年 | 65篇 |
2003年 | 48篇 |
2002年 | 48篇 |
2001年 | 32篇 |
2000年 | 35篇 |
1999年 | 39篇 |
1998年 | 19篇 |
1997年 | 14篇 |
1996年 | 18篇 |
1995年 | 25篇 |
1994年 | 21篇 |
1993年 | 22篇 |
1992年 | 25篇 |
1991年 | 29篇 |
1990年 | 16篇 |
1989年 | 16篇 |
1988年 | 18篇 |
1987年 | 23篇 |
1986年 | 15篇 |
1985年 | 15篇 |
1984年 | 17篇 |
1982年 | 15篇 |
1980年 | 15篇 |
1979年 | 22篇 |
1976年 | 13篇 |
1974年 | 25篇 |
1973年 | 18篇 |
1971年 | 14篇 |
1967年 | 12篇 |
1966年 | 12篇 |
排序方式: 共有2127条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
Laura K. Taylor 《Child Development Perspectives》2020,14(3):127-134
The persistence of intergroup conflicts around the world creates urgency for research on child development in such settings. Complementing what we know about internalizing and externalizing developmental outcomes, in this article, I shift the focus to children’s prosocial behaviors and more specifically, the Developmental Peacebuilding Model (DPM). The DPM makes three main contributions: It (a) integrates a developmental intergroup framework and socioecological perspective with a peace-building paradigm to examine the target and type of children’s prosocial behavior in settings of intergroup conflict; (b) outlines how children’s outgroup prosocial behaviors, which promote constructive change at different levels of social ecology, can be understood as peace building and fostering social cohesion; and (c) has implications for research and global policy. 相似文献
62.
63.
64.
65.
We measured driving performance (lane-keeping errors, driving times, and glances away from the road scene) in a video driving simulator for 24 volunteers who each drove alone on a 10.6-km multicurved course while simultaneously placing calls on a mobile phone subscribed to a voice-activated dialing system. Driving performance also was measured for the same distance while participants manually dialed phone numbers and while they drove without dialing. There were 22% fewer lane-keeping errors (p<.01) and 56% fewer glances away from the road scene (p<.01) when they used voice-activated dialing as compared to manual dialing. Significantly longer driving times in both of the dialing conditions as compared to the No Dialing condition are discussed in terms of the hypothesis that drivers decrease driving speed to compensate for the demands of the secondary phone tasks. 相似文献
66.
Raymond J. Taylor 《Contemporary Family Therapy》2002,24(3):475-481
This is a case study of a family with a child (age eight) with reactive attachment disorder and the subsequent individual and family therapy. Treatment of choice for the child was Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing (EMDR) and supportive educational counseling for the parents and family. Qualitative evaluation of the process demonstrated that the parents observed an instant change in the child's attitude. The child reported that she felt better about family, school, and truthfulness, and stated about the therapy: It opened a window for me. A 12-month evaluation demonstrated continued positive effects. 相似文献
67.
68.
Wells KC Epstein JN Hinshaw SP Conners CK Klaric J Abikoff HB Abramowitz A Arnold LE Elliott G Greenhill LL Hechtman L Hoza B Jensen PS March JS Pelham W Pfiffner L Severe J Swanson JM Vitiello B Wigal T 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2000,28(6):543-553
Parenting and family stress treatment outcomes in the MTA study were examined. Male and female (579), 7–9-year-old children with combined type Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), were recruited at six sites around the United States and Canada, and randomly assigned to one of four groups: intensive, multi-faceted behavior therapy program alone (Beh); carefully titrated and monitored medication management strategy alone (MedMgt); a well-integrated combination of the two (Comb); or a community comparison group (CC). Treatment occurred over 14 months, and assessments were taken at baseline, 3, 9, and 14 months. Parenting behavior and family stress were assessed using parent-report and child-report inventories. Results showed that Beh alone, MedMgt alone, and Comb produced significantly greater decreases in a parent-rated measure of negative parenting, Negative/Ineffective Discipline, than did standard community treatment. The three MTA treatments did not differ significantly from each other on this domain. No differences were noted among the four groups on positive parenting or on family stress variables. Results are discussed in terms of the theoretical and empirically documented importance of negative parenting in the symptoms, comorbidities and long-term outcomes of ADHD. 相似文献
69.
Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight 总被引:29,自引:0,他引:29
Taylor SE Klein LC Lewis BP Gruenewald TL Gurung RA Updegraff JA 《Psychological review》2000,107(3):411-429
The human stress response has been characterized, both physiologically and behaviorally, as "fight-or-flight." Although fight-or-flight may characterize the primary physiological responses to stress for both males and females, we propose that, behaviorally, females' responses are more marked by a pattern of "tend-and-befriend." Tending involves nurturant activities designed to protect the self and offspring that promote safety and reduce distress; befriending is the creation and maintenance of social networks that may aid in this process. The biobehavioral mechanism that underlies the tend-and-befriend pattern appears to draw on the attachment-caregiving system, and neuroendocrine evidence from animal and human studies suggests that oxytocin, in conjunction with female reproductive hormones and endogenous opioid peptide mechanisms, may be at its core. This previously unexplored stress regulatory system has manifold implications for the study of stress. 相似文献
70.
An "aha" effect in memory was first reported by Auble, Franks, and Soraci (1979). They demonstrated that recall was greater for sentences that were initially incomprehensible but which were eventually comprehended, as compared with sentences that were understood from the outset. The present studies extend this "aha" effect to memory for pictorial stimuli. In Experiment 1, a recall advantage for pictures encoded by connecting the dots as compared with those encoded by tracing or visual scanning occurred only in the absence of foreknowledge of the picture (i.e., an "aha" effect). In Experiment 2, we replicated this finding and obtained evidence that conceptually based, verbal foreknowledge does not function in a similar manner as does pictorial foreknowledge in suppressing the "aha" recall advantage. These results place important constraints on previous research on generation effects for visual stimuli and attest to the cross-modal generalizability of the "aha" effect. 相似文献