全文获取类型
收费全文 | 396篇 |
免费 | 23篇 |
专业分类
419篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 4篇 |
2022年 | 4篇 |
2021年 | 3篇 |
2020年 | 11篇 |
2019年 | 14篇 |
2018年 | 21篇 |
2017年 | 17篇 |
2016年 | 22篇 |
2015年 | 6篇 |
2014年 | 15篇 |
2013年 | 47篇 |
2012年 | 32篇 |
2011年 | 26篇 |
2010年 | 10篇 |
2009年 | 13篇 |
2008年 | 15篇 |
2007年 | 17篇 |
2006年 | 15篇 |
2005年 | 18篇 |
2004年 | 10篇 |
2003年 | 17篇 |
2002年 | 9篇 |
2001年 | 10篇 |
2000年 | 8篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1998年 | 5篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1991年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1971年 | 1篇 |
1969年 | 1篇 |
1966年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有419条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
21.
Melinda A. Green Tara K. Ohrt Christine Nguyen Katherine Blasko Sanjeev Khatiwada Amanda Martin 《Basic and applied social psychology》2013,35(3):259-271
The purpose of the present study was to examine heart rate (HR) and affective reactions to state self-objectification as a function of gender. We examined negative affect, positive affect, guilt, and HR at 6-second and 5-minute intervals across baseline, control, high objectification, low objectification, and cologne conditions in men (n = 53) and women (n = 57). Mixed factorial MANOVA results indicated a statistically significant Gender × Condition interaction. Both men and women showed a cardiac orienting response to high versus low objectification. Cardiac stress reactions to objectification were higher among women. Negative affective reactions to objectification were more pervasive across conditions among women. 相似文献
22.
Lia M. Daniels Anne C. Frenzel Robert H. Stupnisky Tara L. Stewart Raymond P. Perry 《The British journal of educational psychology》2013,83(3):396-413
Background and Aims. The literature documents fewer classroom mastery goal structures in secondary school compared to elementary. However, little is known about how personal achievement goals may influence classroom goal structures. This is especially true at the level of pre‐service teachers. Our objective was to investigate if pre‐service teachers’ personal goals predicted their intended classroom goal structures. Sample. Participants were 125 elementary and 175 secondary school pre‐service teachers from two Western Canadian universities. Method. Structural equation modelling was used to examine if the structural relationships and latent means of personal and intended classroom goal structures differed for elementary and secondary school pre‐service teachers. Results. The results revealed that personal goals predicted the goal structures that pre‐service teachers intended to establish; however, the relationships and means differed between elementary and secondary school pre‐service teachers. Specifically, personal mastery‐approach goals positively predicted classroom mastery goals much more strongly at the elementary than the secondary level. Furthermore, elementary pre‐service teachers had significantly higher latent mean scores on personal mastery‐approach goals than their secondary counterparts. Conclusions. It seems possible that the currently documented differences between classroom goal structures noted for elementary compared to secondary school may be based on the personal goals endorsed as pre‐service teachers. The results are further discussed in terms of alignment with research on practising teachers’ personal and classroom goals and implications for teacher education. 相似文献
23.
Hambrick DZ Libarkin JC Petcovic HL Baker KM Elkins J Callahan CN Turner SP Rench TA Ladue ND 《Journal of experimental psychology. General》2012,141(3):397-403
Sources of individual differences in scientific problem solving were investigated. Participants representing a wide range of experience in geology completed tests of visuospatial ability and geological knowledge, and performed a geological bedrock mapping task, in which they attempted to infer the geological structure of an area in the Tobacco Root Mountains of Montana. A Visuospatial Ability × Geological Knowledge interaction was found, such that visuospatial ability positively predicted mapping performance at low, but not high, levels of geological knowledge. This finding suggests that high levels of domain knowledge may sometimes enable circumvention of performance limitations associated with cognitive abilities. 相似文献
24.
Tara?Saathoff-WellsEmail author Rex?E.?Culp Candace?T.?Yancey 《Journal of child and family studies》2005,14(4):487-503
We investigated perceiver bias in relation to children labeled as sexually abused. Building on recent research indicating
that adults perceive children with such a label as having more behavioral problems and lower achievement, we replicated and
expanded upon an earlier study. We tested undergraduate students (N = 699), who judged a six-year old child's aggressive behavior
in vignettes that varied on severity of the aggressive behavior, child gender, and family history (sexually abused, mother
dying of cancer, normal). Perceiver bias was related to family history label for internalizing behavior problems and competence
issues and child gender for internalizing behavior problems and externalizing behavior problems. Implications for educators
and practitioners are discussed as well as routes for future research. 相似文献
25.
Tara Kennedy 《Frontiers of Philosophy in China》2016,11(3):463-482
This paper describes the phenomenological ethics implicit in Heidegger’s later work. It is argued that these phenomenological ethics take the form of a perfectionist ethics in which one consciously resists the temptation to nihilistically enframe other entities as Bestand. Despite Heidegger’s reputation as an inferior animal philosopher, it is then argued that we can employ this ethics to improve our relationship with non-human animals. Specifically, our use of them in the agricultural setting is examined to determine whether or not our current practices are ethical according to Heidegger’s normative model. Ultimately it is concluded that, more often than not, animals are harmed both ontically and ontologically by our modern farming practices. We are called on instead to try to dwell meditatively with other entities, to be-with them in such a way that respects them as inexhaustibly meaningful instantiations of being as such. This requires changes to the way in which we satisfy our needs as consumers. 相似文献
26.
The authors examined the suppression of spider-related thoughts in spider-fearful (n = 23) and nonfearful (n = 22) individuals. Participants were primed with vivid pictures of spiders and a story about spiders. Next, they were randomly assigned to 1 of 2 conditions: (a) suppression of thoughts associated with the previously presented spider-related stimuli or (b) free expression of any thoughts, including those related to the spider-related stimuli. All participants completed a subsequent free-expression exercise. Results indicated that spider-fearful individuals expressed thoughts about the spider-related stimuli for a longer length of time than did nonfearful individuals, particularly in the suppression condition. Participants in both groups demonstrated a rebound of thoughts associated with the spider-related stimuli following suppression. The authors propose that the priming of feared stimuli makes suppression of fear-related thoughts particularly difficult for fearful individuals, perhaps by activating a state of heightened arousal. 相似文献
27.
Stoinski TS Whiten A 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》2003,117(3):272-282
Increasing evidence for behavioral differences between populations of primates has created a resurgence of interest in examining mechanisms of information transfer between individuals. The authors examined the social transmission of information in 15 captive orangutans (Pongo abelii and Pongo pygmaeus) using a simulated food-processing task. Experimental subjects were shown 1 of 2 methods for removing a suite of defenses on an "artificial fruit." Control subjects were given no prior exposure before interacting with the fruit. Observing a model provided a functional advantage in the task, as significantly more experimental than control subjects opened the fruit. Within the experimental groups, the authors found a trend toward differences in the actual behaviors used to remove 1 of the defenses. Results support observations from the wild implying horizontal transfer of information in orangutans and show that a number of social learning processes are likely to be involved in the transfer of knowledge in this species. 相似文献
28.
Johnson JD Simmons C Trawalter S Ferguson T Reed W 《Personality & social psychology bulletin》2003,29(5):609-622
Experiment 1 indicated that when the White supervisor's negative treatment of a Black subordinate was unconstrained, participant race had no impact on attributions. Conversely, when the treatment was constrained, Black participants reported greater racist attributions than did White participants. Experiment 2 indicated that when the supervisor reported no response or a minimal negative response (i.e., indicating that he did not support his actions) after his negative treatment of the Black subordinate, Black participants reported greater racist attributions than did White participants. Conversely, when the supervisor's negative treatment was followed by a more extreme negative response, participant race had no impact on attributions. Experiment 3 indicated that Black participants were less likely than White participants to perceive a minimal negative response as reflecting a White supervisor's lack of support for his negative actions. Conversely, participant race had no impact on attributions of a Black supervisor's negative actions. 相似文献
29.
30.
Anne McDonald Culp Rex E. Culp Tamara Hechtner‐Galvin Christina S. Howell Tara Saathoff‐Wells Pam Marr 《Infant mental health journal》2004,25(1):1-15
Professionals in the field of child development were employed to provide home visitation services to 156 mothers and infants prenatally through the infants' first birthdays. Compared to a control group of mothers (n = 107), the intervention mothers utilized more community resources, had safer homes, had more appropriate developmental expectations, had better understanding of noncorporal punishment, and had behaviors that were more accepting and respectful to their infants. The home visitors were well educated, with experience in home visitation, and supervised. Program implementation procedures were documented and supervised. Program fidelity and the efficacy of using child development specialists as home visitors are discussed in regards to the positive findings. ©2004 Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health. 相似文献