首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   546篇
  免费   25篇
  2023年   5篇
  2020年   8篇
  2019年   7篇
  2018年   10篇
  2017年   9篇
  2016年   16篇
  2015年   11篇
  2014年   9篇
  2013年   62篇
  2012年   18篇
  2011年   19篇
  2010年   15篇
  2009年   12篇
  2008年   18篇
  2007年   18篇
  2006年   22篇
  2005年   13篇
  2004年   16篇
  2003年   16篇
  2002年   13篇
  2001年   6篇
  2000年   11篇
  1999年   10篇
  1998年   8篇
  1997年   5篇
  1995年   7篇
  1994年   5篇
  1993年   4篇
  1992年   7篇
  1991年   10篇
  1990年   5篇
  1988年   9篇
  1987年   6篇
  1986年   8篇
  1985年   6篇
  1984年   10篇
  1983年   4篇
  1981年   6篇
  1980年   5篇
  1979年   9篇
  1978年   10篇
  1977年   4篇
  1976年   4篇
  1975年   8篇
  1974年   10篇
  1973年   11篇
  1971年   5篇
  1968年   7篇
  1967年   4篇
  1966年   8篇
排序方式: 共有571条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
121.
122.
123.
A fifteen-year-old severely disturbed girl was treated for aphonia. Because of the extent of her withdrawal, the subject was conditioned in a laboratory setting and received tokens for speaking loudly enough to operate a voice-operated relay. Conditioning at first consisted of saying 100 monosyllabic words, with the possibility of reinforcement on each word. Later, the subject was required to say a polysyllabic word, and finally, five or six words per token. The subject was shaped to speak with normal loudness in the laboratory, and generalization to a reading situation in the laboratory was measured and observed to occur, at first for a few minutes, and later for a longer period. Generalization to a reading situation in the classroom did not occur, but the subject's voice loudness also increased in the classroom when several new reinforcement contingencies were put into effect there.  相似文献   
124.
Tests of proprioceptive shift (PS), visual shift (VS), and negative aftereffect (NA) were made during 25-min exposure to 20-D displacement and during a subsequent 30-min dark decay period in two separate experiments. Different groups of subjects explored hallways or viewed their active hand during exposure. VS was greatest in hall exposure, while PS was greatest in hand exposure. Larger VS occurred in the second experiment, where test procedures were modified to minimize a tendency to center the target within the momentary or remembered field of view. Substantial and possibly complete VS decay occurred when the initial level of adaptation was high, but although PS decay was substantial, it was not complete. In all conditions, the sum of VS and PS numerically exceeded the NA, and this difference tended to be largest and significant in the hall exposure. Implications of this effect for the two-component additivity hypothesis are discussed.  相似文献   
125.
Two experiments examined the relationship between conditioning to the CS and background using a novel CER paradigm, in which a long background stimulus played the role of more conventional contextual cues. Experiment 1 manipulated the probability of US occurrence given the CS (p(US/CS)). Conditioning to the background was not a monotonically decreasing function of p(US/CS) at all shock intensities, and conditioning to the CS was remarkably insensitive to the value of p(US/CS) when assessed off the baseline. Experiment 2 manipulated the trace interval between the CS and US. Although conditioning to the CS decreased as the trace interval increased, conditioning to the background was dependent upon whether it served as the interstimulus interval (ISI; interval between the CS and US) or intertrial interval (ITI; interval between CS-US pairs) stimulus. Conditioning to the ISI background decreased as the trace interval increased, but conditioning to the ITI background at first increased, but then decreased as the trace interval was further increased. These results are discussed with respect to the adequacy of contemporary models of conditioning.  相似文献   
126.
The possible relationship between hypnotic susceptibility and familiar handedness was examined. In a mass-testing session of students enrolled in introductory psychology classes, subjects were administered the Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form A and were also required to complete a questionnaire that ascertained information on their dominant handedness and that of their immediate family relatives. Subjects who had immediate sinistral relatives scored significantly lower in hypnotic susceptibility compared to those who had a history of familial dextrality. When immediate relatives of the original subject pool were tested on hypnotic susceptibility level, sinistral relatives scored lower in susceptibility than dextral relatives. This may indicate the existence of a familial component in hypnotic susceptibility.  相似文献   
127.
Equal-loudness contours were first obtained for five stimulus frequencies at four stimulus intensities. These 20 stimuli were then presented as reaction-time signals in a Donders C paradigm. The Z-transform method of convolution, as applied in linear systems identification, was used to deconvolve an empirically generated response (or “residual”) distribution (TR) from each of the 20 reaction-time (RT) distributions obtained at different intensities and frequencies. The resulting sensory-detection (td) models formed exponential densities at strong intensities (60 and 80 phons), but their shapes were either gamma or normal at relatively weak intensities (20 and 40 phons). Our analyses support the idea that the simple reactiontime process (RT) is a convolution (or sum) of two component stages: stimulus detection (td), followed by response evocation (tr). Based on the shapes of td, a neural-impulse theory is offered to account for the detection of simple auditory RT signals.  相似文献   
128.
To examine the interrelationships of MMPI scale scores among low back pain patients, 50 profiles were scored on the traditional 13 scales, Hanvik's Lb scale and seven subscales derived from Scales 1 and 3. The profiles were randomly divided into two equal samples and factor analyzed. Comparison of the two samples yielded three common factors. The first factor measured severity of the symptoms, the second assessed the level of anger and aggression and the third assessed the psychogenic components of the syndrome. The implications of the factors are discussed.  相似文献   
129.
A major problem for the premarital counselor is that the literature in the field is scattered throughout numerous journals and books representing several disciplines. Since counselors tend to subscribe to and read the literature from their own academic discipline, they may be unaware of what is being written elsewhere. This article contains a bibliography of what the authors consider to be the most relevant books and articles on premarital counseling published by the various disciplines. For the convenience of the reader, each listing is also coded as to its content.The authors express their appreciation to Charles R. Figley, Ph.D., Douglas H. Sprenkle, Ph.D., and Diane Spencer, M.S., for their assistance in the preparation of this paper. The valuable assistance of those contributors who notified the authors of their publications is gratefully acknowledged.  相似文献   
130.
Although Likert ratings of social skill are widely used in behavioral assessment, such ratings may have limited replicability potential and social validity. The present study compared the reliability of Likert and Thurstone Equal-Appearing Interval scales. The Thurstone-derived scale was slightly more reliable than its Likert counterpart. Moreover, the empirical and clinical potential of Thurstone scaling substantially exceeds that of Likert and other rating methods.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号