首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   473篇
  免费   13篇
  486篇
  2016年   4篇
  2015年   4篇
  2013年   5篇
  2012年   5篇
  2011年   10篇
  2010年   8篇
  2009年   9篇
  2008年   10篇
  2007年   13篇
  2006年   4篇
  2005年   8篇
  2004年   6篇
  2000年   7篇
  1998年   9篇
  1997年   12篇
  1996年   7篇
  1995年   10篇
  1994年   8篇
  1993年   7篇
  1992年   12篇
  1991年   9篇
  1990年   8篇
  1989年   15篇
  1988年   8篇
  1987年   14篇
  1986年   14篇
  1985年   13篇
  1984年   11篇
  1983年   6篇
  1982年   18篇
  1981年   14篇
  1980年   11篇
  1979年   15篇
  1978年   7篇
  1977年   6篇
  1976年   9篇
  1975年   6篇
  1974年   13篇
  1973年   12篇
  1972年   5篇
  1971年   10篇
  1970年   6篇
  1969年   10篇
  1968年   4篇
  1967年   6篇
  1966年   9篇
  1958年   12篇
  1957年   9篇
  1956年   11篇
  1955年   9篇
排序方式: 共有486条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
111.
112.
113.
114.
115.
116.
117.
118.
119.
Two experiments were conducted to test hypotheses derived from complexity theory regarding the interactive effect of situational variables and cognitive complexity of a communicator on the informational value of written communications. Complexity theory suggests that cognitively complex encoders, utilizing more dimensions of judgment in recognizing differences, are better able than cognitively simple encoders to distinguish the intended meaning of a communication from all competing, but inaccurate interpretations. Further, evidence exists which indicates that affective bias toward the referent of communication may lessen differences due to cognitive complexity. Much controversy has developed over the specific form of the interaction effect. Results of the experiments indicate that the superiority of cognitively complex encoders over cognitively simple ones is not limited by affective stimulus valence.  相似文献   
120.
The purpose was to evaluate methods for selecting respondents who would respond accurately to items on a job-analysis questionnaire. One general method involved obtaining from employees measures that assessed background, performance, and organizational information. This information could be used to identify respondents who were knowledgeable about the job and, therefore, able to rate the job accurately. A second general method involved collecting job-analysis data from all potential job-analysis respondents and, on the basis of indices computed on these data, selecting a subsample from them. Two indices were investigated: (1) the D index, which assessed similarity between an individual's ratings and the population's mean ratings, and (2) the carelessness index, which measured an individual's tendency to rate tasks known to be unrelated to the focal job as important. Both methods were applied to a sample of 343 mental-health workers. Four general postulates for job analysts were proposed on the basis of the results: (1) Different selection measures yield somewhat different job-analysis respondents. (2) Respondents are not equally accurate and, with the use of the carelessness index, may be screened for the tendency to make errors. (3) In some applications, the number of sampled respondents needs to be greater than three in order to obtain reliable results. (4) To the degree that the job is ill-defined and unstable, the selection of job-analysis respondents assumes greater importance and is riskier.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号