全文获取类型
收费全文 | 36430篇 |
免费 | 345篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
36778篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 186篇 |
2019年 | 190篇 |
2018年 | 3668篇 |
2017年 | 2994篇 |
2016年 | 2475篇 |
2015年 | 407篇 |
2014年 | 347篇 |
2013年 | 1225篇 |
2012年 | 1053篇 |
2011年 | 2801篇 |
2010年 | 2776篇 |
2009年 | 1730篇 |
2008年 | 2091篇 |
2007年 | 2558篇 |
2006年 | 410篇 |
2005年 | 591篇 |
2004年 | 490篇 |
2003年 | 387篇 |
2002年 | 382篇 |
2001年 | 710篇 |
2000年 | 728篇 |
1999年 | 481篇 |
1998年 | 196篇 |
1997年 | 188篇 |
1996年 | 180篇 |
1995年 | 142篇 |
1992年 | 368篇 |
1991年 | 348篇 |
1990年 | 370篇 |
1989年 | 312篇 |
1988年 | 301篇 |
1987年 | 293篇 |
1986年 | 266篇 |
1985年 | 267篇 |
1984年 | 252篇 |
1983年 | 207篇 |
1982年 | 167篇 |
1979年 | 228篇 |
1978年 | 174篇 |
1977年 | 151篇 |
1975年 | 196篇 |
1974年 | 232篇 |
1973年 | 240篇 |
1972年 | 190篇 |
1971年 | 188篇 |
1970年 | 153篇 |
1969年 | 182篇 |
1968年 | 217篇 |
1967年 | 198篇 |
1966年 | 158篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
781.
Hansen JT 《Psychological reports》2000,87(2):675-676
The Rorschach test is often used to assess object relations. This research examined whether human responses are necessary for a valid assessment. 相似文献
782.
Pragmatic use of stereotyping in teamwork: social loafing and compensation as a function of inferred partner-situation fit 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Using 4 experiments, the authors examined how stereotypic information about teammates influences social loafing and compensation during collective tasks. In each experiment, participants performed better on cognitive tasks when there was a poor (vs. good) fit between the stereotypic strengths of their partner and the requirements of the task. This pattern occurred whether participants used gender stereotypes (Experiment 1) or occupational stereotypes (Experiments 2 to 4) and occurred even when participants only anticipated working on a collective task (Experiment 4). In Experiment 3, the pattern occurred only in the collective (not in the coactive) condition, providing direct evidence for social loafing. Together, these results suggest that people use stereotypes to tune their motivation to optimize the ratio of their own individual effort to the team's expected output. 相似文献
783.
Horner DT 《Perception & psychophysics》2000,62(8):1545-1555
When target patterns and nontarget patterns are presented either to the same or to adjacent locations on the distal pad of the index finger, the amount of interference in identifying targets depends on both the shape and the location of the nontarget (Horner, 1997). In the present study, the question of whether such interference is caused by masking (the masker in some way distorts the initial representation of the target) or by response competition (the observer mistakenly responds with the masker, rather than with the target) was investigated. A 4-to-2 paradigm was used (Craig, 1995), in which four stimuli were mapped to only two responses. Targets and nontargets were randomly selected from the set of four stimuli and presented to the same or adjacent locations on the same fingerpad. Both the distal pad and the medial pad of the index finger were tested, because innervation density varies proximodistally on the distal pad, but not on the medial pad. The results indicated that response competition was an important factor limiting perception. Furthermore, perception was affected by varying location on the distal pad, but not on the medial pad. Finally, varying location on the distal pad affected perception only when responses were based on pattern shape, not when responses were based on direction of motion. The results are discussed in terms of differences in innervation density between adjacent locations and possible resultant differences in the spatial filtering properties of the skin. 相似文献
784.
The ability of younger and older adults to perceive the 3-D shape, depth, and curvature of smooth surfaces defined by differential motion and binocular disparity was evaluated in six experiments. The number of points defining the surfaces and their spatial and temporal correspondences were manipulated. For stereoscopic sinusoidal surfaces, the spatial frequency of the corrugations was also varied. For surfaces defined by motion, the lifetimes of the individual points in the patterns were varied, and comparisons were made between the perception of surfaces defined by points and that of more ecologically valid textured surfaces. In all experiments, the older observers were less sensitive to the depths and curvatures of the surfaces, although the deficits were much larger for motion-defined surfaces. The results demonstrate that older adults can extract depth and shape from optical patterns containing only differential motion or binocular disparity, but these abilities are often manifested at reduced levels of performance. 相似文献
785.
Emotional states and physical health 总被引:30,自引:0,他引:30
Positive emotional states may promote healthy perceptions, beliefs, and physical well-being itself. To explore potential mechanisms linking pleasant feelings and good health, the authors consider several lines of research, including (a) direct effects of positive affect on physiology, especially the immune system, (b) the information value of emotional experiences, (c) the psychological resources engendered by positive feeling states, (d) the ways in which mood can motivate health-relevant behaviors, and (e) the elicitation of social support. As anticipated by the Greek physician Hippocrates, positive emotions and healthy outcomes may be linked through multiple pathways. 相似文献
786.
The peer review process, whether formally applied in publication and grant review, or informally, such as exchange of ideas in scientific and professional newsgroups, has sparked controversy. Writers in this area agree that scholarly reviews that are inappropriate in tone are not uncommon. Indeed, commentators have suggested rules and guidelines that can be used to improve the review process and to make reviewers more accountable. In this paper, we examine the relevance and impact of ethical codes on the conduct of peer review. It is our contention that the peer review process can be improved, not by a new set of rules but through closer attention to the ethical principles to which we, as psychologists, already subscribe. 相似文献
787.
Scott A. Edwards J. Maria Bermúdez Donna L. Canady Howard O. Protinsky 《Contemporary Family Therapy》2000,22(2):223-232
A therapy research team interviewed a therapist in the presence of a couple. Following each of six consecutive sessions with a couple, an interviewer posed questions to the therapist and couple. While one question was standard to each interview, other questions were generated by team members who tracked the entire process via live observation. These questions were directed first at the therapist, then at the couple, and then focused on therapeutic process and outcome. Findings of this process for the therapy system included decreasing the hierarchy within the client/therapist relationship; increasing the client ownership and energy in the therapy process; discovering the presence of parallel processes between the systems; and informing the direction of future therapy sessions. Practical, clinical implications are discussed for a variety of therapy settings. 相似文献
788.
Kim NG Fajen BR Turvey MT 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2000,26(1):31-56
Five experiments examined circular heading perception with optical flows that departed from the canonical form. Noncanonicity was achieved through nonrigidity of the environment (Experiments 1 and 2), oscillations of the point of observation (Experiment 3), and the bending of light (Experiments 4 and 5). In Experiments 1 and 2, perception was impaired more by nonrigidity of the ground plane than by nonrigidity of the medium. In Experiment 3, perception was unimpaired by noncanonical flows induced by the bounce and sway of observer locomotion. In Experiments 4 and 5, perception was not impaired when light paths were distorted by a spherical projection, but perception was impaired when they were distorted by a sine function. Results are discussed in relation to the hypothesis that the information for perceiving heading is the ordinal pattern of optical flow. 相似文献
789.
790.
We report the case of a patient, LEW, who presents with modality-specific naming deficits. He is seriously impaired in naming pictures of both objects and actions. His naming to auditory verbal definitions and of actions carried out by the experimenter is, however, relatively well preserved. He has no visual perceptual deficits and his access to the semantics of pictures is as good as that to the semantics of spoken words. While LEW is not an optic aphasic patient, his pattern of performance is relevant to the debate that has taken place of the organization of the semantic system. We discuss his case from this perspective and argue that LEW's selective deficits support the multiple semantics position. We also argue that the "preverbal message" level in the speech production model of Levelt (1989) is the equivalent of "verbal semantics." We provide additional constraints and principles to the concept of the preverbal message and we term the system so constrained the "restricted preverbal message." Copyright 2000 Academic Press. 相似文献