首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   74838篇
  免费   2875篇
  国内免费   28篇
  2020年   820篇
  2019年   931篇
  2018年   2395篇
  2017年   2324篇
  2016年   2080篇
  2015年   1060篇
  2014年   1245篇
  2013年   5742篇
  2012年   2518篇
  2011年   3066篇
  2010年   2248篇
  2009年   1879篇
  2008年   2601篇
  2007年   2800篇
  2006年   1800篇
  2005年   1608篇
  2004年   1577篇
  2003年   1485篇
  2002年   1428篇
  2001年   2367篇
  2000年   2268篇
  1999年   1668篇
  1998年   767篇
  1997年   667篇
  1996年   735篇
  1995年   653篇
  1994年   653篇
  1993年   624篇
  1992年   1373篇
  1991年   1277篇
  1990年   1262篇
  1989年   1186篇
  1988年   1156篇
  1987年   1081篇
  1986年   1055篇
  1985年   1096篇
  1984年   930篇
  1983年   793篇
  1982年   611篇
  1979年   948篇
  1978年   700篇
  1975年   766篇
  1974年   873篇
  1973年   898篇
  1972年   756篇
  1971年   717篇
  1969年   629篇
  1968年   787篇
  1967年   677篇
  1966年   641篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
Logical connectives, such as “AND”, “OR”, “IF . . . THEN”, and “IF AND ONLY IF” are ubiquitous in both language and cognition; however, reasoning with logical connectives is error-prone. We argue that some of these errors may stem from people's tendency to minimize the number of possibilities compatible with logical connectives and to construct a “minimalist” one-possibility representation. As a result, connectives denoting a single possibility (e.g., conjunctions) are likely to be represented correctly, whereas connectives denoting multiple possibilities (e.g., disjunctions or conditionals) are likely to be erroneously represented as conjunctions. These predictions were tested and confirmed in three experiments using different paradigms. In Experiment 1, participants were presented with a multiple-choice task and asked to select all and only those possibilities that would indicate that compound verbal propositions were true versus false. In Experiment 2, a somewhat similar task was used, except that participants were asked later to perform a cued recall of verbal propositions. Finally, Experiment 3 used an old/new recognition paradigm to examine participants' ability to accurately recognize different logical connectives. The results of the three experiments are discussed in relation to theories of representation of possibilities and theories of reasoning.  相似文献   
62.
63.
64.
The ability to make egocentric distance estimates of a single point source of light, seen in darkness and without the cues of changing size and luminance, was investigated in sixteen observers. The attenuation required to maintain constant luminance, when the target was viewed from different distances, was shown to follow the inverse square law providing the angle subtended by the light was less than 20 s arc. Distance changes were also simulated by means of a split mirror which produced vergence cues, or by test lenses to provide accommodation cues. Over the range 0.5 to 9.2 m distance estimates were surprisingly accurate, although there was some overestimation of near and underestimation of far distances. Most observers made good judgements when only convergence cues were varied, whereas no observers made consistently good judgements when only accommodation cues were varied. The difficulties are discussed in terms of the accommodation-convergence link. When distance was simulated by changing convergence and accommodation cues, estimates were not as good as when real distance was changed. Since good estimates were made with brief target exposures, these judgements were not based on subsequent convergence or accommodation changes. It is suggested that the metric or reference against which the apparently absolute judgements were made was the efferent demand signal associated with a 'resting' position of convergence in darkness.  相似文献   
65.
66.
67.
68.
69.
70.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号