全文获取类型
收费全文 | 40367篇 |
免费 | 786篇 |
国内免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
41158篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 418篇 |
2018年 | 594篇 |
2017年 | 598篇 |
2016年 | 643篇 |
2015年 | 484篇 |
2014年 | 605篇 |
2013年 | 2841篇 |
2012年 | 1059篇 |
2011年 | 1064篇 |
2010年 | 750篇 |
2009年 | 685篇 |
2008年 | 997篇 |
2007年 | 967篇 |
2006年 | 894篇 |
2005年 | 812篇 |
2004年 | 766篇 |
2003年 | 730篇 |
2002年 | 780篇 |
2001年 | 1262篇 |
2000年 | 1238篇 |
1999年 | 941篇 |
1998年 | 421篇 |
1997年 | 436篇 |
1996年 | 425篇 |
1995年 | 398篇 |
1992年 | 809篇 |
1991年 | 761篇 |
1990年 | 757篇 |
1989年 | 688篇 |
1988年 | 689篇 |
1987年 | 655篇 |
1986年 | 685篇 |
1985年 | 666篇 |
1984年 | 606篇 |
1983年 | 529篇 |
1982年 | 399篇 |
1979年 | 603篇 |
1978年 | 485篇 |
1977年 | 418篇 |
1976年 | 423篇 |
1975年 | 520篇 |
1974年 | 639篇 |
1973年 | 669篇 |
1972年 | 502篇 |
1971年 | 523篇 |
1970年 | 466篇 |
1969年 | 524篇 |
1968年 | 627篇 |
1967年 | 575篇 |
1966年 | 559篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
251.
Participants completed the Rotter internal/external (IE) instrument using three different response formats before and after completion of the Outward Bound program, and were evaluated by external observers at the end of the intervention. Multitrait-multimethod analyses indicated that five specific IE facets identified in previous research were consistently distinguished with each of the response formats. While responses were substantially more internal after the intervention, effect sizes varied with the IE facet and with the response format. Observer responses were significantly correlated with self-responses, and provided additional support for the construct validity of responses to the Rotter instrument and the interpretation of the intervention effect. Nevertheless, problems with the Rotter instrument were identified, and the implications for further research were discussed. 相似文献
252.
J. Douglas Rodick Scott W. Henggeler Cindy L. Hanson 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1986,14(1):77-87
Two basic assumptions of the Circumplex Model of family systems were evaluated: (1) Healthy families evidence balanced degrees of cohesion and adaptability, whereas problem families evidence extreme degrees, and (2) families with balanced degrees of cohesion and adaptability possess more positive communication skills than families with extreme degrees. Subjects were 58 mother-son dyads from father-absent families. In 29 of the families the adolescent was a juvenile offender, while in the remaining families there was no history of arrest or psychiatric referral. The dyads completed the Family Adaptability and Cohesion Scales (FACES) and an unrevealed differences interaction task. FACES was highly proficient at differentiating delinquent from nondelinquent families. Moreover, in families with balanced degrees of cohesion and adaptability, the mothers' communication was significantly more supportive and explicit, and the dyads evidenced significantly greater warmth and affection than dyads with extreme degrees.This research was supported by funding to the Department of Psychology at Memphis State University granted through the Centers of Excellence Program of the State of Tennessee. We thank the staff of the Memphis-Metro Youth Diversion Project for their consideration and assistance. 相似文献
253.
254.
T O Nelson R J Leonesio R S Landwehr L Narens 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》1986,12(2):279-287
Distinctions are drawn between different predictors of an individual's memory performance, with emphasis on the notion of privileged access to idiosyncratic knowledge. Research is reported in which undergraduates attempted to recall the answers to general-information questions, then made feeling-of-knowing judgments on nonrecalled items, and subsequently had a criterion test (relearning, perceptual identification, or one of two versions of recognition). For predicting an individual's criterion performance, the individual's own feeling-of-knowing predictions were intermediate between two kinds of normative predictions: The individual's feeling-of-knowing predictions were more accurate than predictions derived from normative feeling-of-knowing ratings but were less accurate than predictions derived from base-rate item difficulty (normative probabilities of correct recall). Subsidiary analyses showed that factors other than unreliability are responsible for the partial inaccuracy of the individual's feeling of knowing. Ramifications are discussed for possible ways to improve the accuracy of an individual's feeling-of-knowing predictions. 相似文献
255.
256.
257.
This study examined the relationship of parental education, race, and gender to sex-role stereotyping in five-year-old kindergartners. A significant effect for race and parental education level was noted. White children gave more stereotyped responses than did black children, and children whose parents were in the middle- and high-educational levels gave more stereotyped responses than did children whose parents were in the low-educational level. A significant interaction was also found between educational level of parents and race. White children tended to give more stereotyped responses as the educational level of their parents increased; this trend was not evident for black children. No significant differences in stereotyping were noted between the sexes.To whom requests for reprints or information concerning the Bardwell-Sietsema Sex Stereotype Scale should be addressed at Department of Elementary Education, East Texas State University, Commerce, Texas 75428. 相似文献
258.
Younger adult students between 19 and 24 years of age (M = 21.3 years), older adult students between 61 and 76 years of age (M = 67.9 years), and older adult nonstudents between 62 and 76 years of age (M = 68.5 years) were assessed for health (self-ratings of physical and mental health), social functioning (self-ratings of physical and mental activity, perceived role activity level, perceived roles, locus of control, and age-norm expectations), and cognitive functioning (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised vocabulary and block design, and paired associate memory). Age differences were observed in self-ratings of health, social roles, intellectual performance, and memory. No student status differences were observed. The results are discussed in terms of plasticity of intellectual function and characteristics of student status in later adulthood. 相似文献
259.
260.
Short scales were developed to measure three broad dimensions of personality in the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES I) Epidemiologic Followup Study. Items to measure neuroticism were selected rationally from the General Well-Being Schedule, and items to measure extraversion and openness to experience were selected by multiple regression from the NEO Personality Inventory. In a sample of 654 men and women from the Augmented Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging, all three short scales showed clear evidence of convergent and discriminant validity against full scales in self-reports, and against peer and spouse ratings. Because they were developed and validated on a sample with a comparable age range, the NHANES scales should be useful in examining age trends in these three personality dimensions in a national sample. 相似文献