全文获取类型
收费全文 | 16324篇 |
免费 | 352篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
16679篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 198篇 |
2019年 | 194篇 |
2018年 | 291篇 |
2017年 | 252篇 |
2016年 | 302篇 |
2015年 | 217篇 |
2014年 | 275篇 |
2013年 | 1140篇 |
2012年 | 499篇 |
2011年 | 452篇 |
2010年 | 328篇 |
2009年 | 294篇 |
2008年 | 414篇 |
2007年 | 435篇 |
2006年 | 385篇 |
2005年 | 370篇 |
2004年 | 319篇 |
2003年 | 277篇 |
2002年 | 306篇 |
2001年 | 678篇 |
2000年 | 670篇 |
1999年 | 459篇 |
1998年 | 172篇 |
1997年 | 169篇 |
1996年 | 166篇 |
1995年 | 139篇 |
1992年 | 363篇 |
1991年 | 343篇 |
1990年 | 358篇 |
1989年 | 306篇 |
1988年 | 290篇 |
1987年 | 284篇 |
1986年 | 259篇 |
1985年 | 263篇 |
1984年 | 245篇 |
1983年 | 208篇 |
1982年 | 165篇 |
1979年 | 225篇 |
1978年 | 174篇 |
1977年 | 153篇 |
1975年 | 196篇 |
1974年 | 236篇 |
1973年 | 233篇 |
1972年 | 185篇 |
1971年 | 182篇 |
1970年 | 152篇 |
1969年 | 176篇 |
1968年 | 211篇 |
1967年 | 193篇 |
1966年 | 157篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
The motion aftereffect (MAE) was measured with retinally moving vertical gratings positioned above and below (flanking) a retinally stationary central grating (experiments 1 and 2). Motion over the retina was produced by leftward motion of the flanking gratings relative to the stationary eyes, and by rightward eye or head movements tracking the moving (but retinally stationary) central grating relative to the stationary (but retinally moving) surround gratings. In experiment 1 the motion occurred within a fixed boundary on the screen, and oppositely directed MAEs were produced in the central and flanking gratings with static fixation; but with eye or head tracking MAEs were reported only in the central grating. In experiment 2 motion over the retina was equated for the static and tracking conditions by moving blocks of grating without any dynamic occlusion and disclosure at the boundaries. Both conditions yielded equivalent leftward MAEs of the central grating in the same direction as the prior flanking motion, ie an MAE was consistently produced in the region that had remained retinally stationary. No MAE was recorded in the flanking gratings, even though they moved over the retina during adaptation. When just two gratings were presented, MAEs were produced in both, but in opposite directions (experiments 3 and 4). It is concluded that the MAE is a consequence of adapting signals for the relative motion between elements of a display. 相似文献
992.
The cognitive model of panic disorder proposes that panic patients interpret physical symptoms in a catastrophic way, thus precipitating panic attacks. This study describes the development of a questionnaire measure designed to assess beliefs about the health implications of various symptoms. Two groups, agoraphobics and normals, were compared on a variety of measures. Agoraphobics showed much higher panic frequency, fear of anxiety, and prevalence of frightening cognitions when anxious, but did not score higher than normals when assessing seriousness of symptoms. The implications of these results are considered. 相似文献
993.
This study compared two different interpretation styles (tentative and absolute), two levels of subjects' reactance (high and low), and gender on the counselor's social influence, willingness to see the counselor, willingness to help, ability to help, and subjects' anger. No significant main effects or interaction were found for the counselor's social influence, but men in contrast to women perceived the counselor as more willing to help and indicated that they were more willing to see the portrayed counselor. Highly reactant subjects were more willing to see the counselor when absolute interpretations were used; they also thought the counselor was more willing to help when absolute interpretations were used, whereas low reactant subjects thought the counselor was more willing to help when tentative interpretations were used. Mixed results were found for subjects' anger. 相似文献
994.
995.
996.
Nancy C. Higgins C. Alec Pollard William T. Merkel 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》1992,11(1):79-85
This exploratory study sought to determine whether selected religion-related factors differentiated between 86 patients with
obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD), 73 patients with panic disorder, and 292 patients with other psychiatric (non-anxiety)
disorders. A standard history questionnaire was used to obtain information from patients concerning religion of origin, involvement
in religious activities, religious conflict, and perceived religiousness of parents. It was found that the percentage of patients
who reported experiencing religious conflict was significantly higher for the OCD group than for the other two groups. Other
findings suggested associations between Catholicism and OCD and between Protestantism and panic disorder, but further research
is needed to clarify these relationships.
This article is based on a paper presented at the 98th annual convention of the American Psychological Association, Boston,
August 1990. 相似文献
997.
998.
999.
Chumaeva N Hintsanen M Ravaja N Puttonen S Heponiemi T Pulkki-Råback L Juonala M Raitakari OT Viikari JS Keltikangas-Järvinen L 《Stress (Amsterdam, Netherlands)》2009,12(4):283-293
The present study examined the interaction between vital exhaustion and cardiac reactivity and recovery on preclinical atherosclerosis assessed by carotid intima-media thickness (IMT) in young men and women. We measured heart rate (HR), respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA), and pre-ejection period (PEP) in response to mental arithmetic and speech tasks. Vital exhaustion and carotid IMT were also measured. Significant associations were observed for men aged 28-37 years, but not for men aged 22-25 years, nor for women in these age groups. It was shown that, among highly exhausted men in the older age group, lower HR reactivity was related to greater IMT. Our results also imply that, among non-exhausted men in this age group, slow HR and RSA recovery after acute stress predicted higher IMT. These results suggest that long-term stress as assessed by vital exhaustion is a risk only if it has resulted in ineffective cardiac stress reactivity. Autonomic imbalance resulting from chronic stress may be the common mechanism linking vital exhaustion and cardiac responsiveness to an increased risk of atherosclerosis. 相似文献
1000.
Perception of emotion is critical for successful social interaction, yet the neural mechanisms underlying the perception of dynamic, audio-visual emotional cues are poorly understood. Evidence from language and sensory paradigms suggests that the superior temporal sulcus and gyrus (STS/STG) play a key role in the integration of auditory and visual cues. Emotion perception research has focused on static facial cues; however, dynamic audio-visual (AV) cues mimic real-world social cues more accurately than static and/or unimodal stimuli. Novel dynamic AV stimuli were presented using a block design in two fMRI studies, comparing bimodal stimuli to unimodal conditions, and emotional to neutral stimuli. Results suggest that the bilateral superior temporal region plays distinct roles in the perception of emotion and in the integration of auditory and visual cues. Given the greater ecological validity of the stimuli developed for this study, this paradigm may be helpful in elucidating the deficits in emotion perception experienced by clinical populations. 相似文献