全文获取类型
收费全文 | 42203篇 |
免费 | 803篇 |
国内免费 | 6篇 |
专业分类
43012篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 430篇 |
2019年 | 464篇 |
2018年 | 608篇 |
2017年 | 619篇 |
2016年 | 695篇 |
2015年 | 535篇 |
2014年 | 638篇 |
2013年 | 2973篇 |
2012年 | 1145篇 |
2011年 | 1125篇 |
2010年 | 745篇 |
2009年 | 725篇 |
2008年 | 1024篇 |
2007年 | 1003篇 |
2006年 | 878篇 |
2005年 | 854篇 |
2004年 | 779篇 |
2003年 | 751篇 |
2002年 | 793篇 |
2001年 | 1299篇 |
2000年 | 1276篇 |
1999年 | 904篇 |
1998年 | 423篇 |
1997年 | 401篇 |
1992年 | 801篇 |
1991年 | 775篇 |
1990年 | 786篇 |
1989年 | 741篇 |
1988年 | 688篇 |
1987年 | 681篇 |
1986年 | 693篇 |
1985年 | 728篇 |
1984年 | 636篇 |
1983年 | 560篇 |
1982年 | 425篇 |
1981年 | 448篇 |
1979年 | 635篇 |
1978年 | 492篇 |
1977年 | 420篇 |
1976年 | 408篇 |
1975年 | 589篇 |
1974年 | 678篇 |
1973年 | 702篇 |
1972年 | 576篇 |
1971年 | 561篇 |
1970年 | 520篇 |
1969年 | 556篇 |
1968年 | 684篇 |
1967年 | 652篇 |
1966年 | 594篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
751.
752.
de Silva and Rachman (1981) have recently suggested that although non-reinforced exposure of a fear-evoking stimulus constitutes a sufficient condition for unlearning fears, it does not represent a necessary condition. This conclusion was reached, in part, by examples they provide which appeared to them to suggest that fear-reduction can occur in the absence of exposure to the fear stimulus. They then call for an extended search for non-exposure types of intervention, especially those involving ‘sources of information’ and ‘expectations’. After reviewing in detail the arguments advanced, the present authors adopt the position that de Silva and Rachman's conclusions are premature and unwarranted. To abandon or lose sight of one of the most effective and perhaps most important therapeutic principles, CS exposure, would be myopic and detrimental to the advancement of effective behavior change. At this point in time what is needed is a more careful analysis of the extinction process especially at the stimulus level so the effectiveness of this principle can be maximized. 相似文献
753.
754.
Response tempo and separable--integral responding: evidence for an integral-to-separable processing sequence in visual perception 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Three experiments were conducted to examine the relation between response tempo and separable-integral responding. A restricted classification paradigm, in which similarity-based (integral) and dimensionally based (separable) classifications were pitted against one another, was used with the dimensions of length and density (all experiments) and size and brightness (Experiment 3). The subjects were college students (Experiments 1 and 2) and kindergarten, second-, and fifth-grade children (Experiment 3). In all three experiments, rapid responding was associated with fewer dimensional and more similarity responses than was slow responding. This result held when response tempo was simply measured (Experiments 1 and 3) and when it was manipulated by the experimenter (Experiment 2). The results were interpreted to be consistent with models of dimensional processing in which holistic, integral processing precedes analytic dimensional processing. 相似文献
755.
Are variations among right-handed individuals in perceptual asymmetries caused by characteristic arousal differences between hemispheres? 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
J Levy W Heller M T Banich L A Burton 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1983,9(3):329-359
We propose that much of the variance among right-handed subjects in perceptual asymmetries on standard behavioral measures of laterality arises from individual differences in characteristic patterns of asymmetric hemispheric arousal. Dextrals with large right-visual-field (RVF) advantages on a tachistoscopic syllable-identification task (assumed to reflect characteristically higher left-hemisphere than right-hemisphere arousal) outperformed those having weak or no visual-field asymmetries (assumed to reflect characteristically higher right-hemisphere than left-hemisphere arousal). The two groups were equal, however, in asymmetries of error patterns that are thought to indicate linguistic or nonlinguistic encoding strategies. For both groups, relations between visual fields in the ability to discriminate the accuracy of performance followed the pattern of syllable identification itself, suggesting that linguistic and metalinguistic processes are based on the same laterally specialized functions. Subjects with strong RVF advantages had a pessimistic bias for rating performance, and those with weak or no asymmetries had an optimistic bias, particularly for the left visual field (LVF). This is concordant with evidence that the arousal level of the right hemisphere is closely related to affective mood. Finally, consistent with the arousal model, leftward asymmetries on a free-vision face-processing task became larger as RVF advantages on the syllable task diminished and as optimistic biases for the LVF, relative to the RVF, increased. 相似文献
756.
Inhibitory avoidance deficit following short-term adrenalectomy in the rat: the role of adrenal catecholamines 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
Impaired retention of an inhibitory avoidance response was observed in rats subjected to adrenalectomy (ADX) up to 120 hr before the single learning trial. Corticosterone substitution failed to normalize this behavioral deficit. Rats ADX 240 hr prior to the learning trial showed a normalized behavior. Adrenomedullectomy (ADXM) 48 or 240 hr before learning caused a similar impairment as in short-term ADX rats. The 240-hr ADX rats subjected to corticosterone substitution showed the same behavioral deficit as short-term ADX rats or ADXM ones. Immediate postlearning subcutaneous injection of adrenaline in a dose range of 0.005-5.0 micrograms/kg or of noradrenaline (0.005-0.5 microgram/kg) to 48-hr ADX rats resulted in a dose-related improvement of later retention behavior. Higher doses of catecholamines were less or ineffective. Postlearning treatment of 48-hr ADXM rats with adrenaline (0.5-500 micrograms/kg) caused a similar pattern of behavioral changes. It is concluded that adrenal catecholamines play an important role in the modulation of consolidation of memory. In addition, the high circulating ACTH levels that follow long-term ADX may correct for the behaviorial deficit induced by the absence of adrenomedullary catecholamines. 相似文献
757.
758.
Prospective study of postpartum depression: 4 1/2-year follow-up of women and children 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
The consequences of maternal postpartum depression for mothers and children were investigated in a 4 1/2-year follow-up study, which included 70 of 99 women who had participated in an earlier study of postpartum depression. Information about maternal adjustment and depression during the follow-up period and child adjustment at age 4 1/2 years was obtained. Women who had experienced a postpartum depression were predicted to be at increased risk for subsequent depression and poor adjustment of their child. Postpartum depression was directly related to subsequent depression but not child problems. Later depression was related to child problems at 4 1/2 years. We concluded that postpartum depression may increase risk for later maternal depression and in turn increases risk for child behavior problems. Intervening with women who have experienced a postpartum depression may reduce likelihood of future depressions and child behavior problems. 相似文献
759.
Hyperventilation (hv) is increasingly recognized as being significant in a number of psychological and medical conditions. The core of treatment for hv is breathing retraining, usually on an individual basis. This article describes a group therapy for breathing retraining for patients with hv-induced panic reactions. An analysis of group process suggests that such treatment is helpful in ways impossible for individual retraining and that further exploration of this modality is warranted. 相似文献
760.