全文获取类型
收费全文 | 40521篇 |
免费 | 771篇 |
国内免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
41300篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 413篇 |
2019年 | 446篇 |
2018年 | 650篇 |
2017年 | 666篇 |
2016年 | 696篇 |
2015年 | 497篇 |
2014年 | 643篇 |
2013年 | 3026篇 |
2012年 | 1150篇 |
2011年 | 1196篇 |
2010年 | 791篇 |
2009年 | 719篇 |
2008年 | 1048篇 |
2007年 | 1039篇 |
2006年 | 944篇 |
2005年 | 876篇 |
2004年 | 808篇 |
2003年 | 743篇 |
2002年 | 830篇 |
2001年 | 1393篇 |
2000年 | 1374篇 |
1999年 | 1009篇 |
1998年 | 444篇 |
1997年 | 403篇 |
1996年 | 391篇 |
1992年 | 809篇 |
1991年 | 755篇 |
1990年 | 797篇 |
1989年 | 711篇 |
1988年 | 673篇 |
1987年 | 683篇 |
1986年 | 657篇 |
1985年 | 678篇 |
1984年 | 582篇 |
1983年 | 508篇 |
1982年 | 417篇 |
1981年 | 375篇 |
1979年 | 617篇 |
1978年 | 464篇 |
1977年 | 413篇 |
1975年 | 548篇 |
1974年 | 609篇 |
1973年 | 582篇 |
1972年 | 510篇 |
1971年 | 464篇 |
1970年 | 422篇 |
1969年 | 501篇 |
1968年 | 585篇 |
1967年 | 524篇 |
1966年 | 495篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
71.
Traits that are influenced by a configuration--rather than by a simple sum--of polymorphic genes may not be seen to be genetic unless one studies monozygotic twins (who share all their genes and thus all gene configurations) because such "emergenic" traits will tend not to run in families. Personal idiosyncrasies that have been found to be surprisingly concordant among MZ twins separated in infancy and reared apart may be emergenic traits. More speculatively, important human traits like leadership, genius in its many manifestations, being an effective therapist or parent, as well as certain psychopathological syndromes may also be emergenic. These ideas reemphasize the importance of the role played in human affairs by genetic variation. 相似文献
72.
Injury is the fourth leading cause of death for all Americans and the most frequent cause of death for those from age 1 to 45. Moreover, injury is associated with higher treatment costs and with greater lost life-years than the other three leading causes of death. Traffic accidents are the leading cause of severe brain injury, including most paraplegic and quadriplegic cases. Because injury control is essentially psychological, there are many opportunities for psychologists to contribute, both to scientific research on the causes of injury and to clinical interventions for injury control. 相似文献
73.
A revised conception of the relationship between psychological science and professional practice is proposed in the light of postmodern changes in perspectives on knowledge. Positivistic science, which has dominated the traditional interpretation of scientist-practitioner training, is considered from a constructivist point of view to be only one possible foundation of psychological knowledge. It is argued that the knowledge base of the profession should be derived with diverse methods and from multiple sources, including the knowledge of practice. The mutuality of science and practice is emphasized. An evolving framework for understanding the epistemology of practice, based on cognitive psychology, is presented. Emphasis on broadened choices of research methods, the development of reflective skills, and better linkage between teaching in the domains of research and practice are urged. Suggestions for research related to scientific training and the knowledge processes of practice are offered. 相似文献
74.
75.
This study tested the effect of using the word stress in the measurement of self-reported occupational stressors and strains. Employees from two organizations responded to a questionnaire that included specific occupational stressors, strains, and 16 items in which the word stress was used. Survey respondents tended to interpret the word stress to refer both to employees' strains or reactions to the work environment and to job stressors or elements of the environment itself. Implications of these findings for occupational stress research are discussed. 相似文献
76.
77.
The multiple and long-term effects of functional communication training relative to a common reductive procedure (time-out from positive reinforcement) were evaluated. Twelve children participated in a functional analysis of their challenging behaviors (Study 1), which implicated adult attention as a maintaining variable. The children were then matched for chronological age, mental age, and language age and assigned to two groups. One group received functional communication training as an intervention for their challenging behavior, and the second group received time-out as a contrast. Both interventions were initially successful (Study 2), but durable results were achieved only with the group that received functional communication training across different stimulus conditions (Study 3). Students whose challenging behaviors were previously reduced with time-out resumed these behaviors in the presence of naive teachers unaware of the children's intervention history. The value of teaching communicative responses to promote maintenance is discussed as it relates to the concept of functional equivalence. 相似文献
78.
79.
Stokes T 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1992,25(2):429-432
80.
D T Horner 《Perception & psychophysics》1992,52(2):201-210
Pairs of vibrotactile patterns were presented to subjects' left middle and index fingerpads (unilateral presentation) or left and right index fingerpads (bilateral presentation), using two Optacon arrays. A set of simple (one-line) patterns and a set of complex (two-line) patterns were constructed so that they were equally identifiable when presented individually. In Experiment 1, discrimination performance was lower for two-line patterns than it was for one-line patterns, and it was lower for unilateral presentation than it was for bilateral presentation. Communality, the number of lines that two patterns share in common, was a major factor in reducing discrimination performance for two-line patterns. Subjects' abilities to identify one member of the pair of patterns were measured in Experiment 2. There were no significant differences in performance between pattern sets or type of presentation when subjects attended to a single pattern. However, when subjects were required to attend to both patterns, identification performance was lower for two-line patterns than it was for one-line patterns, and it was lower for unilateral presentation than it was for bilateral presentation. The results suggest that there are limited attentional resources for processing vibrotactile patterns and that more resources are available bilaterally than are available unilaterally. 相似文献