首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   37835篇
  免费   811篇
  国内免费   10篇
  2021年   153篇
  2020年   248篇
  2019年   231篇
  2018年   3720篇
  2017年   3053篇
  2016年   2552篇
  2015年   473篇
  2014年   420篇
  2013年   1460篇
  2012年   1193篇
  2011年   2875篇
  2010年   2830篇
  2009年   1790篇
  2008年   2164篇
  2007年   2654篇
  2006年   462篇
  2005年   635篇
  2004年   539篇
  2003年   433篇
  2002年   417篇
  2001年   755篇
  2000年   771篇
  1999年   502篇
  1998年   206篇
  1997年   198篇
  1996年   183篇
  1992年   379篇
  1991年   354篇
  1990年   386篇
  1989年   326篇
  1988年   301篇
  1987年   308篇
  1986年   277篇
  1985年   275篇
  1984年   258篇
  1983年   220篇
  1982年   160篇
  1979年   246篇
  1978年   182篇
  1977年   157篇
  1975年   211篇
  1974年   251篇
  1973年   246篇
  1972年   197篇
  1971年   195篇
  1970年   167篇
  1969年   186篇
  1968年   226篇
  1967年   198篇
  1966年   164篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 343 毫秒
71.
This study examined matching-to-sample procedures that might result in the emergence of conditional behavior never explicitly taught. Subjects were preschool children. Two pictures were displayed as comparisons on every trial, and samples were spoken words. In baseline training preceding each of three experiments, children learned to select pictures of a dog, a table, and a banana in response to their spoken English names. Thereafter, probe trials displayed novel comparisons with baseline comparisons: one novel comparison was displayed with the dog and another with the table. The three experiments differed primarily in the nature of the samples presented on probe trials. In Experiment 1, probe samples were novel words, "JAIJAI" and "BREEL." On the probes, each of seven subjects reliably selected the novel comparisons, apparently "excluding" the familiar ones. In Experiment 2, probe samples were from the subjects' baseline. On one probe, for example, the sample was "TABLE," and the subject had to choose either the dog or the novel picture. Exclusion was logically possible because the dog had always before been incorrect in the presence of "TABLE." Under these conditions, however, only two of nine children excluded reliably. In Experiment 3, probe samples were words that had never been samples on any matching-to-sample trial, but that had controlled the children's behavior in other settings. On one probe, for example, the sample was "PENCIL," and the subject had to choose either the dog or a novel picture. Subjects virtually always excluded the former and selected the latter. Unreliable exclusion in Experiment 2, therefore, apparently resulted because the probe samples had previously served also as samples on baseline trials. Spontaneous verbalizations recorded during probing provided further data consistent with this interpretation. The study helps to define variables controlling exclusion performances by showing that such performances are more likely to occur if the sample has no prior experimental history.  相似文献   
72.
We examined the functional role of verbalizations in the generalization of self-instructional training with preschoolers. Children learned to overtly self-instruct during classroom work periods prior to covert training. Data were collected on children's acquisition of verbal regulation during training and on overt use of self-instructions in the classroom generalization setting. Results of a multiple baseline design across subjects indicated that treatment effects were evident in the training setting but did not generalize to the classroom until children were emitting overt self-instructions in the classroom itself. The production of self-verbalizations in the generalization setting was related to changes in correct responding, on-task behavior, and efficiency in completing academic work.  相似文献   
73.
74.
75.
76.
77.
78.
T D Crespi 《Adolescence》1988,23(92):805-811
The effectiveness of a time-out intervention for adolescent psychiatric patients, adjudicated (delinquent) youth, and behaviorally disordered youngsters was explored in this study. The research was conducted in three psychiatric hospitals for children and adolescents, a facility for adjudicated youth, and in a day-treatment program. Utilizing a comparative outcome model, 813 occurrences of time-out with 274 youth were investigated in order to assess levels of effectiveness. The results indicated that time-out had a significant impact, and the use of a child-care specialist as time-out monitor improves overall effectiveness. Important implications for practicing professionals and clinical researchers are noted.  相似文献   
79.
80.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号