全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3860篇 |
免费 | 73篇 |
国内免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
3938篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 5篇 |
2015年 | 4篇 |
2012年 | 454篇 |
2011年 | 482篇 |
2010年 | 91篇 |
2009年 | 57篇 |
2008年 | 390篇 |
2007年 | 404篇 |
2006年 | 372篇 |
2005年 | 288篇 |
2004年 | 261篇 |
2003年 | 240篇 |
2002年 | 206篇 |
2001年 | 131篇 |
2000年 | 182篇 |
1999年 | 78篇 |
1998年 | 17篇 |
1997年 | 12篇 |
1996年 | 7篇 |
1995年 | 7篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 13篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1989年 | 11篇 |
1988年 | 8篇 |
1987年 | 8篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 9篇 |
1984年 | 5篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 12篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1973年 | 5篇 |
1972年 | 6篇 |
1965年 | 5篇 |
1963年 | 6篇 |
1959年 | 6篇 |
1958年 | 20篇 |
1957年 | 13篇 |
1956年 | 11篇 |
1955年 | 7篇 |
1952年 | 5篇 |
1950年 | 5篇 |
1948年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有3938条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
821.
Ogura T 《Animal cognition》2011,14(3):427-431
Contrafreeloading, which means that animals work for food even though identical food is freely available, has been reported
in animals’ feeding behavior. This phenomenon has been assumed to be explained by the information primacy model, in which
the information about a food resource as well as the food itself is valuable for animals. This study confirmed a contrafreeloading-like
phenomenon using movies as rewards rather than food in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) and investigated the motivational system that exists behind contrafreeloading. In the experiment, movies that were presented
dependently on subjects’ responses (earned movies) and movies that were presented automatically (free movies) were supplied
simultaneously. The subjects continued to make responses to obtain the presentation of the earned movies although identical
movies were available as free movies. These results provide the first evidence of contrafreeloading that occurs with movie
rewards. The motivation maintaining the contrafreeloading behavior for movies may be control over the environment according
to the competence theory. 相似文献
822.
823.
We examined the relative weight given to obtained and foregone outcomes (i.e., outcomes from the non-chosen options) in repeated choices using cognitive modeling. Previous modeling studies have yielded mixed results. When participants' choices are analyzed by models that predict the next choice ahead in a sequence of decisions, the results imply that people give less weight to foregone than to obtained outcomes. In contrast, in simulation models of n trials ahead, the results imply that, on average, people give equal weight to foregone and obtained outcomes. Using datasets of experience-based binary choices with fixed (stationary) payoff distributions (Erev & Haruvy, in press) and dynamic (nonstationary) payoff distributions (Rakow & Miler, 2009), we employed generalization tests at the individual level to examine whether the findings derived from the one-step-ahead method are due to overfitting. The results of trial-ahead model fitting implied that for the nonstationary tasks only, foregone outcomes received lower weight. However, when this dataset was assessed via generalization criteria at the individual level, equal weighting of foregone and obtained outcomes was the best assumption. This implies that overfitting is implicated in the superior fit of models that assume discounting of foregone outcomes. 相似文献
824.
825.
Rummel J Boywitt CD Meiser T 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》2011,64(11):2194-2210
The class of multinomial processing tree (MPT) models has been used extensively in cognitive psychology to model latent cognitive processes. Critical for the usefulness of a MPT model is its psychological validity. Generally, the validity of a MPT model is demonstrated by showing that its parameters are selectively and predictably affected by theoretically meaningful experimental manipulations. Another approach is to test the convergent validity of the model parameters and other extraneous measures intended to measure the same cognitive processes. Here, we advance the concept of construct validity (Cronbach & Meehl, 1955 ) as a criterion for model validity in MPT modelling and show how this approach can be fruitfully utilized using the example of a MPT model of event-based prospective memory. For that purpose, we investigated the convergent validity of the model parameters and established extraneous measures of prospective memory processes over a range of experimental settings, and we found a lack of convergent validity between the two indices. On a conceptual level, these results illustrate the importance of testing convergent validity. Additionally, they have implications for prospective memory research, because they demonstrate that the MPT model of event-based prospective memory is not able to differentiate between different processes contributing to prospective memory performance. 相似文献
826.
Leonhard T 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》2011,64(10):2012-2043
Three psychological refractory period (PRP) experiments were conducted to assess the effect of central arrival times at the bottleneck on task order scheduling. In Experiment 1, a visual first task (plus-minus symbol discrimination) was combined with an auditory second task (left-right tone judgement) in a standard PRP paradigm with constant task order. In Experiment 2, the order of the tasks varied unpredictably. In Experiment 3, visual-auditory dual-task trials were randomly mixed with single-task trials. To dissociate central arrival times from stimulus detection times, the perceptual stage of the visual task was extended using stimulus degradation. Most importantly, no evidence for a first-come, first-served principle at the central bottleneck was found with the employed paradigms. Instead, the results indicated that preparation (Experiment 1) and the detection times of the stimuli (Experiments 2 and 3) were the main determinants of central processing order in the present study. In the light of previous research, the results indicate that central processing order can be influenced by various factors. The interplay between these factors seems to depend highly on the conditions and requirements of the employed experimental paradigm. 相似文献
827.
Zittoun T 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》2011,45(3):325-334
Starting from two accounts of psychological change in therapeutic settings, based respectively on the analysis of metaphors (Faccio et al. 2011, this issue) and on transformative moments in self narratives (Ribeiro and Gon?alves 2011, this issue), this paper examines core processes of psychological change. Drawing on sociocultural psychology, the paper first argues that core processes of change in such therapeutic settings take place at the level of the organization of a person's semiotic sets. Second, the paper suggests that such therapeutic frames are likely to provoke changes in other aspects of a person's life as these aim at transforming the persons thinking capacities, and as these have as objects situations external to them. 相似文献
828.
The present study examines the reliability and construct validity of the Dutch version of the Psychopathy Check List: Youth Version (PCL:YV) in a sample of male adolescents admitted to a secure juvenile justice treatment institution (N = 98). Hare's four-factor model is used to examine reliability and validity of the separate dimensions of psychopathy. Interrater reliabilities are good to excellent for the PCL:YV total score and most factor scores, except for the affective factor. Several suggestions are offered for optimizing reliability of this factor. Finally, meaningful associations between PCL:YV scores and scores on the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-Adolescent and the Interpersonal Checklist-Revised support the construct validity of the PCL:YV total score as well as the four factors in the Dutch context. 相似文献
829.
Preverbal infants can represent the causal structure of events, including distinguishing the agentive and receptive roles and categorizing entities according to stable causal dispositions. This study investigated how infants combine these 2 kinds of causal inference. In Experiments 1 and 2, 9.5-month-olds used the position of a human hand or a novel puppet (causal agents), but not a toy train (an inert object), to predict the subsequent motion of a beanbag. Conversely, in Experiment 3, 10- and 7-month-olds used the motion of the beanbag to infer the position of a hand but not of a toy block. These data suggest that preverbal infants expect a causal agent as the source of motion of an inert object. 相似文献
830.
The purposes of the present study were to develop a Parenting Scale of Inconsistency and to evaluate its initial reliability and validity. The 12 items assess the inconsistency among parents' moods, behaviors, and attitudes toward children. In the primary study, 517 participants completed three measures: the new Parenting Scale of Inconsistency, the Parental Bonding Instrument, and the Depression Scale of the General Health Questionnaire. The Parenting Scale of Inconsistency had good test-retest reliability of .85 and internal consistency of .88 (Cronbach coefficient alpha). Construct validity was good as Inconsistency scores were significantly correlated with the Care and Overprotection scores of the Parental Bonding Instrument and with the Depression scores. Moreover, Inconsistency scores' relation with a dimension of parenting style distinct from Care and Overprotection suggested that the Parenting Scale of Inconsistency had factorial validity. This scale seems a potential measure for examining the relationships between inconsistent parenting and the mental health of children. 相似文献