首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   288篇
  免费   1篇
  2016年   1篇
  2013年   26篇
  2012年   1篇
  2011年   1篇
  2009年   1篇
  2008年   4篇
  2007年   2篇
  2006年   2篇
  2004年   2篇
  2003年   3篇
  2002年   2篇
  2001年   1篇
  1999年   2篇
  1998年   1篇
  1997年   3篇
  1996年   1篇
  1995年   3篇
  1994年   1篇
  1993年   1篇
  1992年   2篇
  1991年   10篇
  1990年   5篇
  1989年   6篇
  1988年   2篇
  1987年   3篇
  1986年   4篇
  1985年   17篇
  1984年   16篇
  1983年   38篇
  1982年   16篇
  1981年   18篇
  1980年   11篇
  1979年   8篇
  1978年   10篇
  1977年   5篇
  1976年   3篇
  1975年   6篇
  1974年   7篇
  1973年   4篇
  1972年   8篇
  1971年   8篇
  1970年   3篇
  1969年   4篇
  1968年   1篇
  1967年   3篇
  1966年   6篇
  1965年   1篇
  1964年   2篇
  1955年   1篇
  1952年   1篇
排序方式: 共有289条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
101.
Nine hundred and twenty-one males and 555 females in Uganda completed the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire. Indices of factor comparison indicated that the personality dimensions of P, E, N and L were virtually identical in Uganda and England. Some item changes were required to establish satisfactory reliabilities (alpha) for all factors. Sex differences revealed that males scored higher than females on E but lower on N, which is the usual finding. Strikingly, however, there was no sex difference for P and L, there being, in fact, a very slight tendency for females to score above males on P and below them on L. Cross-cultural comparisons, using only items both Ugandan and English scoring keys had in common, showed Ugandan Ss to score higher on L than their English counterparts, Ugandan males also scoring higher on E and N and Ugandan females scoring higher on P.  相似文献   
102.
103.
Fifty-two subjects were assigned to one of four groups on the basis of scores on the Extraversion scale of the Eysenck Personality Inventory and on the General Activation scale of the Activation-Deactivation Adjective Check List. The subjects learned two lists composed of categorically related groups of words, with the number of categories and the number of words per category varied. Memory was probed by simultaneously presenting the subject with a category name and an item-position cue, and recording the recall latency. The major finding was that activation and extraversion interactively determined the recall latency for both category and item recall. The results were considered in light of arousal theory.  相似文献   
104.
105.
106.
107.
A new theoretical framework for work on anxiety and memory is proposed. Anxious subjects engage in task-irrelevant processing which preempts processing resources and some of the available capacity of working memory. They typically attempt to compensate for the adverse effects of this task-irrelevent processing on task performance by increased effort. It follows from these theoretical assumptions that anxiety will have differential effects on performance efficiency (i.e., the quality of performance) and on processing effectiveness (i.e., performance efficiency/effort). Anxiety will always reduce processing effectiveness, but will not impair performance efficiency if there is sufficient effort expenditure. Consequently, reliance on measures of performance efficiency will often obscure the detrimental effects of anxiety on processing effectiveness.  相似文献   
108.
A study is reported of neurotic patients at St. Bartholomew's Hospital, London, who were given the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire and who were rated by their doctors for prognosis and final assessment after treatment. The results indicated that those patients who had elevated psychoticism scores took longer to improve and that doctors rated them harder to treat at final assessment. It is suggested that early knowledge of patients with a poor prognosis, in the form of their Psychoticism score, might prove helpful to psychiatrists and psychologists, both for diagnosis and (primarily) for choice of appropriate treatment.  相似文献   
109.
110.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号