全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1814篇 |
免费 | 41篇 |
专业分类
1855篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 28篇 |
2019年 | 25篇 |
2018年 | 34篇 |
2017年 | 29篇 |
2016年 | 44篇 |
2015年 | 33篇 |
2014年 | 37篇 |
2013年 | 188篇 |
2012年 | 60篇 |
2011年 | 77篇 |
2010年 | 39篇 |
2009年 | 49篇 |
2008年 | 76篇 |
2007年 | 71篇 |
2006年 | 76篇 |
2005年 | 59篇 |
2004年 | 61篇 |
2003年 | 46篇 |
2002年 | 73篇 |
2001年 | 33篇 |
2000年 | 24篇 |
1999年 | 21篇 |
1998年 | 18篇 |
1997年 | 24篇 |
1996年 | 27篇 |
1995年 | 21篇 |
1994年 | 17篇 |
1993年 | 27篇 |
1992年 | 19篇 |
1991年 | 20篇 |
1990年 | 16篇 |
1989年 | 28篇 |
1987年 | 13篇 |
1986年 | 18篇 |
1985年 | 23篇 |
1984年 | 14篇 |
1982年 | 21篇 |
1981年 | 22篇 |
1980年 | 20篇 |
1979年 | 14篇 |
1978年 | 25篇 |
1977年 | 21篇 |
1976年 | 20篇 |
1975年 | 18篇 |
1974年 | 16篇 |
1973年 | 22篇 |
1972年 | 12篇 |
1969年 | 14篇 |
1968年 | 19篇 |
1966年 | 14篇 |
排序方式: 共有1855条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
181.
182.
Suzanne Gieser 《The Journal of analytical psychology》2014,59(2):165-173
In this paper the city of Copenhagen is used as a starting point to highlight some critical historical events, both concerning the exchange of ideas between the physicist Wolfgang Pauli and C.G. Jung and the history of psychotherapy in Scandinavia. Pauli's years in Copenhagen under the influence of Niels Bohr and his philosophy prepared him mentally to receiveC.G. Jung's ideas. The paper also recounts the one occasion that Jung was in Scandinavia, attending the 9th conference of the General Medical Society for Psychotherapy in Copenhagen, just before going to New York in 1937 to give his final seminars on Pauli's dreams. The reason for focusing on these particular events is that they also constitute part of the history of the reception of psychodynamic psychology in Sweden and Scandinavia. 相似文献
183.
We examined the relation between cognitive development and fear, anxiety, and behavioral inhibition in a non-clinical sample
of 226 Dutch children aged 4–9 years. To assess cognitive development, children were tested with Piagetian conservation tasks
and a Theory-of-Mind (TOM) test. Fears were measured by means of a self-report scale completed by the children, while anxiety
symptoms and behavioral inhibition were indexed by rating scales that were filled out by parents. Significant age trends were
observed for some anxiety phenomena. For example, younger children displayed higher fear scores, whereas older children exhibited
higher levels of generalized anxiety. Most importantly, results of regression analyses (in which we controlled for age) indicated
that cognitive development, and in particular TOM ability, made a unique and significant contribution to various domains of
behavioral inhibition. In all cases, higher levels of TOM were associated with lower levels of behavioral inhibition. In general,
percentages of explained variance were rather small (i.e., <6%), indicating that the role of cognitive development in various
anxiety phenomena is limited. 相似文献
184.
Embodied cognitive science appeals to the idea that cognition depends on the body as well as on the brain. This study looks at whether we are more likely to engage just the brain or enlist the body for complex cognitive functioning such as creative problem solving. Participants were presented with a puzzle based on De Bono's lateral thinking puzzles. The puzzle consisted of rotating and joining two-dimensional shapes to make a three-dimensional one. In one condition, participants were given the choice of either solving the puzzle mentally or through manipulation of the images on a computer screen. In another condition, the subjects had to solve the puzzle first mentally and then report which mode they would have preferred to solve the puzzle. Two more conditions were applied with slight variations. In all conditions, an overwhelming majority of participants chose to solve the puzzle by manipulation, even though there was not a significant increase on performance. It appeared that participants were making a conscious choice for the body to play a feedback-driven role in creative cognitive processing. This strong preference for manual manipulation over just mental representation, regardless of the impact on performance, would seem to suggest that it is our natural tendency to involve the body in complex cognitive functioning. This would support the theory that cognition may be more than just a neural process, and that it is a dynamic interplay between body, brain and world. The experiential feedback of the body moving through space and time may be an inherently important factor in creative cognition. 相似文献
185.
A randomized controlled trial (RCT) of telephone-delivered cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) was compared with usual care for late-life anxiety disorders. Client satisfaction, as assessed by the Client Satisfaction Questionnaire, was high (M = 27.4, range 8-32), and attrition rates were low (8.3%). Preliminary results suggest that participants receiving CBT, compared with usual care, experience declines in general anxiety, worry, anxiety sensitivity, and insomnia. 相似文献
186.
Tatiana Glebova Suzanne Bartle‐Haring Rashmi Gangamma Michael Knerr Robin Ostrom Delaney Kevin Meyer Tiffany McDowell Katie Adkins Erika Grafsky 《Journal of Family Therapy》2011,33(1):42-65
Therapeutic alliance research in couple therapy using multiple perspectives and longitudinal data has been sparse. This study used structural equation modelling to explore relationships between changes in alliance and in progress from clients' and therapists' perspective in a fairly large sample of couples (N=195) during the initial stage of therapy at an on‐campus training clinic. Self‐rated alliance was measured after sessions 2 through 4 with the Working Alliance Inventory. There was very little change in alliance over the early sessions of therapy, and changes in alliance did not always account for changes in relationship satisfaction. Husbands' perceptions of satisfaction and alliance seem to play an important role in the dynamics of the therapeutic process. Findings suggest a reciprocal relationship between perceptions of alliance and progress in therapy when combining perceptions of therapists and couple clients. Clinical implications and future research are discussed. 相似文献
187.
188.
Coping with stress is a major focus for chemically dependent persons in relapse prevention programs. The relationships among
coping and psychological distress were investigated in 71 men with substance use disorders, at both pre—and post-treatment.
It was predicted that low task-oriented coping, high emotion-oriented coping, and high avoidance-oriented coping would predict
psychological distress (e.g., anxiety and depression). It was also expected that use of task-oriented coping would increase
following treatment and that use of emotion-oriented coping and avoidance-oriented coping, and reported levels of psychological
distress would all decrease following treatment. Participants completed the Coping Inventory for Stressful Situations and
the General Health Questionnaire, took part in an intensive treatment program (e.g., relaxation, drug education, stress management)
and completed these questionnaires again following treatment. Results indicated that high emotion-oriented coping predicted
hypochondriasis, anxiety and depression. Task-oriented and avoidance-oriented coping did not predict psychological distress,
although task-oriented coping was negatively related to hypochondriasis, anxiety and depression. The results regarding change
in coping styles and levels of psychological distress were as hypothesized, except that avoidance-oriented coping did not
significantly change following treatment. Theoretical and practical implications of the research are discussed. 相似文献
189.
Recall is typically better for emotional than for neutral stimuli. This enhancement is believed to rely on limbic regions. Memory is also better for neutral stimuli embedded in an emotional context. The neural substrate supporting this effect has not been thoroughly investigated but may include frontal lobe, as well as limbic circuits. Alzheimer's disease (AD) results in atrophy of limbic structures, whereas normal aging relatively spares limbic regions but affects prefrontal areas. The authors hypothesized that AD would reduce all enhancement effects, whereas aging would disproportionately affect enhancement based on emotional context. The results confirmed the authors' hypotheses: Young and older adults, but not AD patients, showed better memory for emotional versus neutral pictures and words. Older adults and AD patients showed no benefit from emotional context, whereas young adults remembered more items embedded in an emotional versus neutral context. 相似文献
190.
Cognitive models of social anxiety implicate various factors in the initiation and maintenance of socially anxious states, including anticipatory processing, self-focused attention and post-event processing. The present study focused on post-event processing, which is a post-mortem analysis following a social event that is described as ruminative in nature and serves to maintain social anxiety. Participants (N=112; 64 women, 48 men) were presented with vignettes that involved making mistakes in public and were instructed to record their thoughts to allow for the examination of the content of post-event processing. Ruminative coping and distraction were assessed via self-report. Results indicated that participants high in social anxiety (n=55) were more likely to ruminate and less likely to distract when faced with socially anxious stressors compared to those low in social anxiety (n=57). Further, as hypothesized, participants high in social anxiety recorded more negative thoughts and more upward counterfactual thoughts ('if only' type thoughts on how things could have been better; associated with negative affect) compared to those low in social anxiety. These results are discussed in terms of cognitive models of social anxiety. 相似文献