全文获取类型
收费全文 | 782篇 |
免费 | 15篇 |
专业分类
797篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 3篇 |
2022年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 7篇 |
2020年 | 14篇 |
2019年 | 15篇 |
2018年 | 19篇 |
2017年 | 17篇 |
2016年 | 27篇 |
2015年 | 20篇 |
2014年 | 28篇 |
2013年 | 84篇 |
2012年 | 39篇 |
2011年 | 43篇 |
2010年 | 16篇 |
2009年 | 29篇 |
2008年 | 42篇 |
2007年 | 37篇 |
2006年 | 40篇 |
2005年 | 30篇 |
2004年 | 31篇 |
2003年 | 23篇 |
2002年 | 24篇 |
2001年 | 8篇 |
2000年 | 7篇 |
1999年 | 10篇 |
1998年 | 11篇 |
1997年 | 8篇 |
1996年 | 14篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 11篇 |
1993年 | 13篇 |
1992年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 9篇 |
1990年 | 6篇 |
1989年 | 12篇 |
1988年 | 5篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 11篇 |
1982年 | 8篇 |
1981年 | 11篇 |
1980年 | 8篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 7篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1971年 | 1篇 |
1970年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有797条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
141.
Suzanne B. Graney Rebecca S. Martínez Melissa Bergstrom 《Journal of School Psychology》2009,47(2):121-142
This study examined patterns of growth across benchmark assessments for curriculum-based measures (CBM) over 2 academic years, with the twofold purpose of replicating earlier findings of growth patterns on R-CBM and conducting a preliminary investigation of growth patterns on M-CBM and CBM-Maze. The sample included 898 cases from 3rd through 5th grade over 2 academic years from one elementary school in the rural Midwest. All students participated in tri-annual benchmark assessments in which they were administered R-CBM, CBM-Maze, and M-CBM. Repeated measures analysis of variance was used to examine patterns of growth for all three measures. Students demonstrated greater R-CBM weekly growth during winter-to-spring than fall-to-winter across grade levels. Patterns for CBM-Maze and M-CBM were somewhat inconsistent across school years and grade levels. Results are discussed in terms of contextual variables that may impact within-year growth patterns and technical characteristics of CBM slopes. 相似文献
142.
143.
144.
145.
Suzanne Cunningham 《Synthese》1989,80(2):223-241
Earlier versions of this paper were read in the Philosophy department at the University of Helsinki, in the Institute for Advanced Studies in the Humanities at the University of Edinburgh, at Vanderbilt University, at the Indiana Philosophical Association, and at the Institute for Logic and Cognitive Science at the University of Houston. I am grateful for very helpful comments on each of these occasions, and I am especially appreciative to John Haugeland, Mark Johnson, and George McClure for their comments and suggestions. Thanks, too, to Robert Solomon who urged me to amplify my claims about perceptual meaning. 相似文献
146.
Barrie B. Alexander Suzanne B. Johnson Randy L. Carter 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1984,12(2):199-207
The Family Adaptability and Cohesion Evaluation Scales (FACES), a measure of family functioning, contains three scales: cohesion, adaptability, and social desirability. These were slightly modified (FACES- R) on the basis of pilot data and were administered to 42 clinic families (families seeking or receiving psychological services) and 206 nonclinic families. Support for the scales was found in high coefficient alpha reliabilities and similarity of the sample means and standard deviations to those of the standardization sample. However, there was little agreement among family members' scale scores. Therapists' ratings of cohesion and adaptability did not correlate with scale scores among clinic families. There was no difference between clinic and nonclinic samples on the cohesion or adaptability scales, although the social desirability scale did discriminate between groups. Factor analysis suggested that cohesion, adaptability, and social desirability cannot be clearly differentiated using this measure.This work was supported by the American Association of University Women and by Grant R01HD13820 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. 相似文献
147.
Recent meta-analyses have shown that adding hypnosis enhances the effectiveness of cognitive-behavioral psychotherapy. This hypnotic enhancement effect was evaluated in the analogue treatment of pain. Individuals scoring in the high (n = 135) and low (n = 150) ranges of hypnotic suggestibility were randomly assigned to 1 of 6 conditions: Stress Inoculation Training, the same treatment provided hypnotically, nonhypnotic analgesia suggestions, hypnotic analgesia suggestions, a hypnotic induction treatment, or a control condition. The 5 analogue treatments reduced experimental pain more than the control condition, but were not different from one another. Under circumstances optimized to detect an enhancement effect, neither Stress Inoculation Training nor analgesia suggestions produced more relief when delivered in a hypnotic context than identical treatments provided nonhypnotically. 相似文献
148.
149.
This paper reviews the literature on cognitive tuning sets: the expectations of having to transmit or receive information.
It is shown that having to transmit information leads to a unified and distorted summary being transmitted, a reluctance to
receive additional information, a rejection of inconsistent information, and a shaping of the material to accommodate the
anticipated audience. As well, the opinions of the transmitters become polarized. It is argued that the receiver condition
is probably not the correct control group to use since the outcome depends on the uncontrolled perceived goals. It is suggested
that future research should probably treat the multiple effects of cognitive tuning sets separately. An integrated explanation
is given in terms of anticipating the consequences of communication.
This work was supported by a grant from the Australian Research Grants Scheme. 相似文献
150.
This study examines whether the first group member to advocate a position in the group publically has disproportionate influence on the group decision, and whether group discussion by computer mail or face-to-face determines the extent of the first advocate's influence. Possible explanations of first advocacy influence were tested in an experiment that allowed simultaneous observation of three independent variables: (1) assignment of first advocate (self-selected/randomly assigned), (2) early discussion before advocacy (discussion/no discussion), and (3) mode of communication (face-to-face/electronic mail). It was assumed that a first advocate effect exists if the group choice is closer to the first advocates' stated positions than to the group's average pregroup preferences. An “influence” hypothesis predicts that first advocates who self-select will be closer to the group decision than average pregroup preferences because these are first advocates most likely to persuade other members of the group. A “listening” hypothesis predicts that first advocates who hear an early group discussion will be closer to the group decision than average pregroup preferences because these first advocates are able to anticipate the group decision as a result of listening to what is communicated in group discussion. The results indicated strong support for the “listening” hypothesis. When groups held early discussions before someone advocated a position, the first advocate effect was observed; when groups did not hold early discussions but began their task by having someone advocate a position, there was no first advocate effect. The data suggest that the content and tone of electronic group discussions was qualitatively different from face-to-face group discussions, but the process of group decision making in both conditions was about the same. The implications of these results for group discussion and computer-mediated group decision making in organizations are discussed. 相似文献