全文获取类型
收费全文 | 957篇 |
免费 | 18篇 |
专业分类
975篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 15篇 |
2023年 | 6篇 |
2022年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 11篇 |
2020年 | 22篇 |
2019年 | 24篇 |
2018年 | 32篇 |
2017年 | 34篇 |
2016年 | 33篇 |
2015年 | 22篇 |
2014年 | 36篇 |
2013年 | 123篇 |
2012年 | 65篇 |
2011年 | 55篇 |
2010年 | 26篇 |
2009年 | 23篇 |
2008年 | 31篇 |
2007年 | 39篇 |
2006年 | 32篇 |
2005年 | 28篇 |
2004年 | 32篇 |
2003年 | 19篇 |
2002年 | 20篇 |
2001年 | 17篇 |
2000年 | 17篇 |
1999年 | 9篇 |
1998年 | 9篇 |
1997年 | 11篇 |
1996年 | 6篇 |
1995年 | 6篇 |
1994年 | 10篇 |
1993年 | 10篇 |
1992年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 10篇 |
1989年 | 7篇 |
1987年 | 7篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 13篇 |
1984年 | 6篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 6篇 |
1979年 | 4篇 |
1975年 | 6篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1973年 | 9篇 |
1971年 | 4篇 |
1967年 | 4篇 |
1966年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有975条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
831.
The visuospatial negative priming effect—that is, the slowed-down responding to a previously ignored location—is partly due to response inhibition associated with the previously ignored location (Buckolz, Goldfarb, & Khan, Perception & Psychophysics 66:837-845 2004). We tested whether response inhibition underlies spatial negative priming in the auditory modality as well. Eighty participants localized a target sound while ignoring a simultaneous distractor sound at another location. Eight possible sound locations were arranged in a semicircle around the participant. Pairs of adjacent locations were associated with the same response. On ignored repetition trials, the probe target sound was played from the same location as the previously ignored prime sound. On response control trials, prime distractor and probe target were played from different locations but were associated with the same response. On control trials, prime distractor and probe target shared neither location nor response. A response inhibition account predicts slowed-down responding when the response associated with the prime distractor has to be executed in the probe. There was no evidence of response inhibition in audition. Instead, the negative priming effect depended on whether the sound at the repeatedly occupied location changed identity between prime and probe. The latter result replicates earlier findings and supports the feature mismatching hypothesis, while the former is compatible with the assumption that response inhibition is irrelevant in auditory spatial attention. 相似文献
832.
Klaus Fiedler Yaakov Kareev Judith Avrahami Susanne Beier Florian Kutzner Mandy Hütter 《Memory & cognition》2016,44(1):143-161
Detecting changes, in performance, sales, markets, risks, social relations, or public opinions, constitutes an important adaptive function. In a sequential paradigm devised to investigate detection of change, every trial provides a sample of binary outcomes (e.g., correct vs. incorrect student responses). Participants have to decide whether the proportion of a focal feature (e.g., correct responses) in the population from which the sample is drawn has decreased, remained constant, or increased. Strong and persistent anomalies in change detection arise when changes in proportional quantities vary orthogonally to changes in absolute sample size. Proportional increases are readily detected and nonchanges are erroneously perceived as increases when absolute sample size increases. Conversely, decreasing sample size facilitates the correct detection of proportional decreases and the erroneous perception of nonchanges as decreases. These anomalies are however confined to experienced samples of elementary raw events from which proportions have to be inferred inductively. They disappear when sample proportions are described as percentages in a normalized probability format. To explain these challenging findings, it is essential to understand the inductive-learning constraints imposed on decisions from experience. 相似文献
833.
Denham SA Warren-Khot HK Bassett HH Wyatt T Perna A 《Journal of experimental child psychology》2012,111(3):386-404
The importance of early self-regulatory skill has seen increased focus in the applied research literature given the implications of these skills for early school success. A three-factor latent structure of self-regulation consisting of compliance, cool executive control, and hot executive control was tested against alternative models and retained as best fitting. Tests of model equivalence indicated that the model held invariant across Head Start and private child-care samples. Partial invariance was supported for age and gender. In the validity model, because of a substantial amount of shared variance among latent factors, we included a second-order factor explaining the two types of executive control. Higher order executive control positively predicted teacher report of learning behaviors and social competence in the classroom. These findings are discussed in light of their practical and theoretical significance. 相似文献
834.
835.
Felicity Brock Kelcourse M.Min. M.Phil. Barry Ulanov Claude Barbre Margaret Klenk M.Div. Kathleen Ford C.S.W. Stephen L. Cook Ph.D. Mark Finn Ph.D. Robert W. Gunn D.Min. S. J. Angelo Paiano Melissa A. Gallivan Robert W. Gunn D.Min. Jill Carlen Kirby M.S.Ed. C.S.W. Jorge L. Abreu C.S.W. Jill Carlen Kirby M.S.Ed. C.S.W. Donald F. Dal Maso C.S.W. Irwin B. Blatt Ph.D. Ella Fitzgerald 《Journal of religion and health》1996,35(3):255-277
836.
837.
D. F. Buckle M.B. B.S. D.P.M. P. H. Cook Ph.D. 《Journal of personality assessment》2013,95(4):159-165
838.
839.
840.