全文获取类型
收费全文 | 5432篇 |
免费 | 246篇 |
专业分类
5678篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 29篇 |
2021年 | 33篇 |
2020年 | 68篇 |
2019年 | 92篇 |
2018年 | 144篇 |
2017年 | 122篇 |
2016年 | 145篇 |
2015年 | 109篇 |
2014年 | 123篇 |
2013年 | 569篇 |
2012年 | 235篇 |
2011年 | 223篇 |
2010年 | 177篇 |
2009年 | 144篇 |
2008年 | 244篇 |
2007年 | 263篇 |
2006年 | 213篇 |
2005年 | 167篇 |
2004年 | 158篇 |
2003年 | 175篇 |
2002年 | 185篇 |
2001年 | 120篇 |
2000年 | 102篇 |
1999年 | 115篇 |
1998年 | 88篇 |
1997年 | 70篇 |
1996年 | 89篇 |
1995年 | 70篇 |
1994年 | 61篇 |
1993年 | 60篇 |
1992年 | 90篇 |
1991年 | 65篇 |
1990年 | 72篇 |
1989年 | 57篇 |
1988年 | 55篇 |
1987年 | 56篇 |
1986年 | 60篇 |
1985年 | 64篇 |
1984年 | 69篇 |
1983年 | 53篇 |
1982年 | 70篇 |
1981年 | 56篇 |
1980年 | 47篇 |
1979年 | 47篇 |
1978年 | 46篇 |
1977年 | 52篇 |
1976年 | 46篇 |
1975年 | 36篇 |
1974年 | 35篇 |
1973年 | 28篇 |
排序方式: 共有5678条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
31.
Laurie Chassin Susan Ferencz Stager Richard David Young 《American journal of community psychology》1985,13(4):449-465
Educably mentally retarded (EMR) students and nonhandicapped students from their mainstream classes completed semantic differential ratings of a stereotypic popular teen-ager, juvenile delinquent, and special education student. Subjects also rated their global self-concepts and situation-specific self-concepts within the mainstream and the special class settings. Ratings were done at the beginning (Time 1) and the end (Time 2) of a semester. Results showed that mainstream classes did not "de-label" EMR students. Within the mainstream class, the number of EMR students who saw themselves as similar to a special education student significantly increased over time. Moreover, at Time 2, EMR students were more likely to think of themselves as similar to a special education student in their mainstream class than in their special education class. However, EMR subjects' global self-concepts did not change. Implications for mainstreaming are discussed. 相似文献
32.
33.
D. Robin Hill David S. Krantz Richard J. Contrada Susan M. Hedges Jeffrey A. Ratliff-Grain 《Journal of applied social psychology》1987,17(8):679-698
This study examined the effects of academic stress upon components of Type A behavior and cardiovascular reactivity. In a longitudinal design, heart rate and blood pressure responses to the Type A Structured Interview (SI) and additional laboratory challenges were measured in medical students at three points during an academic semester. Sessions 1 and 3 were scheduled during vacation periods; Session 2 took place during an intensive examination week. Results indicated that three “stylistic” components of Pattern A derived from the SI—loud and explosive speech, short response latency, and potential for hostility—increased significantly during the exam period. Similar increases achieved marginal significance for another stylistic component, rapid and accelerated speech. By contrast, global Type A assessments did not increase with academic demands. Cardiovascular reactivity was significantly but only moderately stable across the three sessions, showing less consistency than has been reported in previous research. In addition, there was little evidence in this study that cardiovascular reactivity was potentiated by academic stress or by Type A behavior. These results demonstrate the influence of naturalistic environmental factors on overt behavioral components that have recently been suggested as coronary risk factors. Therefore, it is advisable for future experimental and epidemiological research to examine the social and environmental context in which these behaviors occur. 相似文献
34.
35.
Differences in academic performance at school depending on handedness: matter for neuropathology? 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
S M Williams 《The Journal of genetic psychology》1987,148(4):469-478
Two studies found that left-handed children had significantly poorer academic records than did right-handed children, though the effect was small. Study 1 provided evidence that, for some academic disciplines, the important difference appeared to be between those children whose family history of handedness was concordant with their own handedness and those children whose family history of handedness was discordant with their own handedness. The inclusion of gender in asymmetry research was bolstered by Study 2. When predicting idiosyncrasies in left-handers' cognitive performance, it should be borne in mind that left-handers form a minority group as well as a neurologically distinct one. 相似文献
36.
This paper calls for a new approach to the measurement of gender similarities and differences in personality. Critiques of current measures that are based on the measurement of stereotypical masculinity and femininity are reviewed, and an alternative measure is presented. It avoids many of the problems in the other scales by measuring expressiveness and instrumentality in such a way that they are not confounded with variables such as independence or autonomy. Results with this measure in five different samples are presented. These confirm our theoretical definitions of expressiveness and instrumentality, and suggest that only the expressive or relational dimension consistently differentiates samples of women and men.We thank Mary Rothbart for her insightful and constructive comments on an earlier draft; Leisha Sanders, Marcia DeCaro, Vickie Van Nortwick, and Lyn Cogswell for their clerical assistance; the Center for the Study of Women in Society for financial support; and Liza Kuecker and Patricia Gwartney-Gibbs for providing data used in the analysis. An earlier version of this paper was presented at the 1984 annual meeting of the Pacific Sociological Association. 相似文献
37.
David E. Sandberg Anke A. Ehrhardt Claude A. Mellins Susan E. Ince Heino F. L. Meyer-Bahlburg 《Sex roles》1987,16(11-12):649-668
The term pioneer is used for girls aspiring to male-dominated careers. This longitudinal study assesses the career choices of 68 middle-class girls during childhood (8–13 years, Phase I) and again during adolescence (13–18 years, Phase II), and investigates the relation between pioneering career choice and personal and family characteristics. Subjects were originally selected for exposure vs nonexposure to exogenous hormones during pregnancy. In childhood, pioneers were somewhat older, were more often persistent tomboys, had higher IQs, and had parents with higher educational attainment. In adolescence, paternal education was the only variable that showed the same relation to career choice. Adolescent girls who had wanted to get married at the time of the childhood assessment were now less likely to choose pioneering careers. Adolescent pioneers also had higher educational aspirations than nonpioneers and less likely anticipated being married within ten years than nonpioneers. A number of statistically borderline relationships were also observed: pioneers had somewhat fewer older brothers than nonpioneers, less often came from homes in which traditional femininity was stressed, and more frequently had mothers who had worked outside the home throughout their daughters' school years than mothers of nonpioneers. Prenatal exposure to exogenous sex hormones was not related to career choice during either childhood or adolescence. 相似文献
38.
Stephen M. Williams 《Current Psychology》1987,6(2):148-154
Previous experimental investigation of the effects of repeating an unfamiliar stimulus suggests that mere exposure breeds
attraction (e.g., Zajonc, 1968). On the other hand, correlational work with naturally occurring stimuli such as names, music,
or landscapes suggests that there is also an overexposure effect: the preference function does rise with familiarity at first
but then reaches a turning point and diminishes. The study (N=72) demonstrates this inverted-U relationship in an experimental setting. The stimuli were synthetic nonsense speech, permitting
exact control of exposure durations and interstimulus intervals. The critical factors for demonstrating the effect are probably
(1) the inclusion of a large number of repetitions, and (2) blocked repetition of each stimulus in a homogeneous sequence
not interspersed with other more or less frequent stimuli. 相似文献
39.
This is a report of the completion responses for 40 three-letter word stems (e.g., ABO) produced by 100 undergraduate students at the University of California at San Diego. The report includes a list of the different words that were written as stem completions, their frequency of occurrence as completions, and their frequency of occurrence in English according to published norms. This list of materials is useful for the construction of completion tests for future investigations of direct priming phenomena, and it may provide an important covariate in the analysis of performance on completion tests. Preliminary analyses of the results reveal three primary factors that determine overall performance on a stem-completion test: word frequency, word length, and meanings per word. Analyses of the completions produced for each individual word stem, however, reveal that usually only one of these factors makes a significant contribution to performance. 相似文献
40.
Susan Kavaler 《American journal of psychoanalysis》1986,46(4):336-344
Repeated object loss drove Sylvia Plath to create, and what she created were her greatest, and best-known poems, collected in the book Ariel. Yet the compulsion to create alternated with the compulsion to destroy. Her suicidal impulses were as intense as her poetic impulses (see Bach, 1985, on the "Marquis de Sade"). How these two contrasting impulses counter-balanced each other, until self-destruction took the lead, is the story of Sylvia Plath. What remains with us is the puzzle of her despair. This puzzle can only be pieced together by looking at the internal object relations of the father-daughter bond, which germinated like a ghost and spector within Sylvia Plath's soul. 相似文献