全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3520篇 |
免费 | 131篇 |
专业分类
3651篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 18篇 |
2021年 | 19篇 |
2020年 | 40篇 |
2019年 | 57篇 |
2018年 | 83篇 |
2017年 | 78篇 |
2016年 | 94篇 |
2015年 | 81篇 |
2014年 | 83篇 |
2013年 | 359篇 |
2012年 | 159篇 |
2011年 | 154篇 |
2010年 | 125篇 |
2009年 | 108篇 |
2008年 | 155篇 |
2007年 | 184篇 |
2006年 | 144篇 |
2005年 | 120篇 |
2004年 | 117篇 |
2003年 | 133篇 |
2002年 | 140篇 |
2001年 | 66篇 |
2000年 | 50篇 |
1999年 | 69篇 |
1998年 | 62篇 |
1997年 | 54篇 |
1996年 | 60篇 |
1995年 | 40篇 |
1994年 | 43篇 |
1993年 | 43篇 |
1992年 | 54篇 |
1991年 | 44篇 |
1990年 | 49篇 |
1989年 | 35篇 |
1988年 | 30篇 |
1987年 | 32篇 |
1986年 | 36篇 |
1985年 | 38篇 |
1984年 | 50篇 |
1983年 | 36篇 |
1982年 | 50篇 |
1981年 | 39篇 |
1980年 | 32篇 |
1979年 | 31篇 |
1978年 | 20篇 |
1977年 | 29篇 |
1976年 | 26篇 |
1975年 | 14篇 |
1974年 | 17篇 |
1973年 | 14篇 |
排序方式: 共有3651条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
101.
Michael F. Dorman Diane Kewley-Port Susan Brady M. T. Turvey 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》1977,29(3):483-497
The recognition of brief vowels was studied in forward and backward masking tasks. In a series of experiments in which both target and mask parameters were systematically varied, two populations of subjects were identified. The majority (Nonmaskers) evidenced little masking at any interstimulus interval, while relatively fewer subjects (Maskers) evidenced backward masking (but not forward masking) over a 100-200 ms interval. Increasing target set size systematically increased masking for the Maskers but not for the Nonmaskers. Adding white noise to the targets increased the extent of masking for both groups. These results suggest (1) that masking does not impose a substantive constraint on vowel perception in running speech, and (2) that multiple strategies may exist for vowel recognition. 相似文献
102.
Susan E. Wiant 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》1977,56(4):234-234
103.
Five parents of nonverbal children were trained in two home settings to modify antecedents and consequences to their children's vocalizations. Generalization effects of the parent training on both the parent's and children's behaviors under different stimulus conditions were investigated using multiple-baseline designs. Increases in parent prompting and reinforcing their children's vocalizations generalized only minimally to a new setting in the home where parent training had not occurred. Child increases in vocalizations produced by the parents in the training settings did generalize to this new setting in the home. There was minimal generalization of child vocalizations to a free-play setting at school. In a formal speech session conducted by a behavior specialist at school, only one child showed definite increases in acquisition rate as a function of the parents starting to train the sound at home. 相似文献
104.
The effect of incongruent color words on speed of classifying ink colors was measured in visual scanning tasks and in card sorting tasks. In both cases, little or no interference effects were noted when the classification allowed focusing on a single ink color or a set of highly similar colors (adjacent hues). Substantial interference occurred when the task required grouping of three dissimilar colors (nonadjacent hues). These findings suggest that the relative efficiency of name and visual codes in making perceptual classifications is largely dependent upon the memory requirements imposed by the task. 相似文献
105.
Susan Oyama 《Journal of psycholinguistic research》1976,5(3):261-283
Immigrants who had learned English at various ages and who had been in the United States for various amounts of time were judged for degree of accent in English. It was found that age at arrival was a strong predictor of degree of accent, while length of stay had very little effect. Other practice and motivational factors were related to accent only by virtue of their correlation with age at arrival. It was suggested that a sensitive period exists for the acquisition of a nonnative phonological system.This article was adapted from a doctoral dissertation for the Department of Psychology and Social Relations, Harvard University, 1973 (unpublished), which was supported by NSF Grant No. GS-36263 and the Radcliffe Institute. 相似文献
106.
Susan Shott 《Journal of the history of the behavioral sciences》1976,12(1):39-46
Many of the concepts central to symbolic interactionism were anticipated by the eighteenth century Scottish moralists. The symbolic-interactionist and Scottish moralist orientations both hold that society alone engenders uniquely human qualities, self arises through sympathetic interaction, and mind and self reconstruct their environments. George H. Mead's conception of thought as internal dialogue between the “I” and “me” aspects of the self and his notion of the “generalized other” were foreshadowed by some of the Scottish moralists, particularly Adam Smith. These schools differ, though, in their treatments of emotion, communication, political structures, and the origin of sympathy. 相似文献
107.
People pursue goals for a variety of reasons, including reasons that take into account close relationships (termed relationally autonomous reasons, or RARs). Two longitudinal studies examined the degree to which relational self-construal, RARs, and personally autonomous reasons (PARs) predicted goal attainment. In Study 1, 166 participants rated 7 goals on several goal outcomes at 2 sessions. Results revealed that self-construal was positively associated with RARs and that RARs predicted goal attainment, controlling for PARs. Study 2 (N = 177) added a 3rd follow-up to the Study 1 design, and results showed perceived progress toward one's goals predicted enhanced RARs but not enhanced PARs. Both studies showed that RARs are an effective motivational component in goal pursuit and attainment. 相似文献
108.
Three experiments explored different schedules of retrieval practice in young adults, older adults, and individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer type. In each experiment, an initial acquisition phase was presented in which participants studied or attempted to retrieve response words to cues, followed by a later cued-recall test. Experiment 1 produced a benefit of expanded retrieval over equal-interval retrieval during acquisition, but this benefit was lost in final cued recall. In Experiments 2 and 3, participants received corrective feedback during acquisition and modified spacing schedules. There was again no evidence of a difference between expanded and equal-interval conditions in final cued recall. Discussion focuses on the potential benefits and costs of expanded retrieval on a theoretical and applied level. 相似文献
109.
The antecedents of mothers' emotional and cognitive responses to infant distress were examined. Participants were 67 mothers and their infants. Mothers completed questionnaires assessing their experiences in the family of origin and current marital relationships both pre‐ and postnatally and their coping strategies prenatally. Infant temperament was observed at 6 months, and mothers were interviewed about their emotional and cognitive responses to infant distress 2 years later to assess their emotional competencies (i.e., accurate identification of negative emotions, emotion efficacy, emotional responses to infant distress, and emotion goals). A childhood history of emotional rejection was negatively associated with empathy and efficacy and positively associated with negative emotions. The association between childhood history and some emotional competencies was moderated by current marital dysfunction, engaged coping, and positive intervening relationships. Maternal marital styles and coping strategies and infant temperament correlated with emotional competencies. Theoretical and clinical implications are discussed from an attachment theory perspective. 相似文献
110.
Executive functions and achievements in school: Shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
St Clair-Thompson HL Gathercole SE 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》2006,59(4):745-759
Links have recently been established between measures of educational attainment and both verbal and visuo-spatial aspects of working memory. Relationships have also been identified between specific executive functions—shifting, updating, and inhibition—and scholastic achievement. In the present study, scholastic attainment, shifting, updating, inhibition, and verbal and visuo-spatial working memory were assessed in 11- and 12-year-old children. Exploratory factor analysis identified two executive factors: one associated with updating functions and one associated with inhibition. Updating abilities were closely linked with performance on both verbal and visuo-spatial working memory span tasks. Working memory was closely linked with attainment in English and mathematics, and inhibition was associated with achievement in English, mathematics, and science. Domain-specific associations existed between verbal working memory and attainment in English, and between visuo-spatial working memory and attainment in English, mathematics and science. Implications of the findings for the theoretical analysis of executive functioning, working memory and children's learning are discussed. 相似文献