全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3569篇 |
免费 | 135篇 |
专业分类
3704篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 18篇 |
2021年 | 19篇 |
2020年 | 42篇 |
2019年 | 59篇 |
2018年 | 85篇 |
2017年 | 85篇 |
2016年 | 95篇 |
2015年 | 84篇 |
2014年 | 85篇 |
2013年 | 371篇 |
2012年 | 160篇 |
2011年 | 156篇 |
2010年 | 125篇 |
2009年 | 108篇 |
2008年 | 156篇 |
2007年 | 186篇 |
2006年 | 145篇 |
2005年 | 120篇 |
2004年 | 118篇 |
2003年 | 133篇 |
2002年 | 140篇 |
2001年 | 66篇 |
2000年 | 52篇 |
1999年 | 70篇 |
1998年 | 62篇 |
1997年 | 55篇 |
1996年 | 60篇 |
1995年 | 40篇 |
1994年 | 43篇 |
1993年 | 43篇 |
1992年 | 54篇 |
1991年 | 43篇 |
1990年 | 49篇 |
1989年 | 35篇 |
1988年 | 31篇 |
1987年 | 33篇 |
1986年 | 36篇 |
1985年 | 38篇 |
1984年 | 50篇 |
1983年 | 36篇 |
1982年 | 51篇 |
1981年 | 40篇 |
1980年 | 32篇 |
1979年 | 34篇 |
1978年 | 20篇 |
1977年 | 30篇 |
1976年 | 26篇 |
1975年 | 15篇 |
1974年 | 17篇 |
1973年 | 14篇 |
排序方式: 共有3704条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
101.
Two field studies were conducted to assess the effect of signs describing how to report a shoplifting on bystander intervention to a staged theft. In the first study, signs providing directions and one of three rationales for reporting had a small effect on attitudes toward reporting and no effect on intervention. In the second study, a definition of the situation as a shoplifting by a confederate had a strong influence on subjects reporting, but presence or absence of signs describing how to report had no impact. Differences between interpersonal and nonpersonal influences are described, and implications for informational campaigns to increase crime reporting are discussed. 相似文献
102.
Susan Karp Manning 《Behavior research methods》1977,9(6):495-498
A set of rating data was collected which compared the visual similarity of consonants to the auditory similarity of the same letters. Analysis of the rating patterns raises important questions about the use of similarity data in drawing conclusions about mode of memory coding in a variety of situations. 相似文献
103.
Michael F. Dorman Diane Kewley-Port Susan Brady M. T. Turvey 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》1977,29(3):483-497
The recognition of brief vowels was studied in forward and backward masking tasks. In a series of experiments in which both target and mask parameters were systematically varied, two populations of subjects were identified. The majority (Nonmaskers) evidenced little masking at any interstimulus interval, while relatively fewer subjects (Maskers) evidenced backward masking (but not forward masking) over a 100-200 ms interval. Increasing target set size systematically increased masking for the Maskers but not for the Nonmaskers. Adding white noise to the targets increased the extent of masking for both groups. These results suggest (1) that masking does not impose a substantive constraint on vowel perception in running speech, and (2) that multiple strategies may exist for vowel recognition. 相似文献
104.
Susan E. Wiant 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》1977,56(4):234-234
105.
Five parents of nonverbal children were trained in two home settings to modify antecedents and consequences to their children's vocalizations. Generalization effects of the parent training on both the parent's and children's behaviors under different stimulus conditions were investigated using multiple-baseline designs. Increases in parent prompting and reinforcing their children's vocalizations generalized only minimally to a new setting in the home where parent training had not occurred. Child increases in vocalizations produced by the parents in the training settings did generalize to this new setting in the home. There was minimal generalization of child vocalizations to a free-play setting at school. In a formal speech session conducted by a behavior specialist at school, only one child showed definite increases in acquisition rate as a function of the parents starting to train the sound at home. 相似文献
106.
The effect of incongruent color words on speed of classifying ink colors was measured in visual scanning tasks and in card sorting tasks. In both cases, little or no interference effects were noted when the classification allowed focusing on a single ink color or a set of highly similar colors (adjacent hues). Substantial interference occurred when the task required grouping of three dissimilar colors (nonadjacent hues). These findings suggest that the relative efficiency of name and visual codes in making perceptual classifications is largely dependent upon the memory requirements imposed by the task. 相似文献
107.
Susan Oyama 《Journal of psycholinguistic research》1976,5(3):261-283
Immigrants who had learned English at various ages and who had been in the United States for various amounts of time were judged for degree of accent in English. It was found that age at arrival was a strong predictor of degree of accent, while length of stay had very little effect. Other practice and motivational factors were related to accent only by virtue of their correlation with age at arrival. It was suggested that a sensitive period exists for the acquisition of a nonnative phonological system.This article was adapted from a doctoral dissertation for the Department of Psychology and Social Relations, Harvard University, 1973 (unpublished), which was supported by NSF Grant No. GS-36263 and the Radcliffe Institute. 相似文献
108.
Susan Shott 《Journal of the history of the behavioral sciences》1976,12(1):39-46
Many of the concepts central to symbolic interactionism were anticipated by the eighteenth century Scottish moralists. The symbolic-interactionist and Scottish moralist orientations both hold that society alone engenders uniquely human qualities, self arises through sympathetic interaction, and mind and self reconstruct their environments. George H. Mead's conception of thought as internal dialogue between the “I” and “me” aspects of the self and his notion of the “generalized other” were foreshadowed by some of the Scottish moralists, particularly Adam Smith. These schools differ, though, in their treatments of emotion, communication, political structures, and the origin of sympathy. 相似文献
109.
People pursue goals for a variety of reasons, including reasons that take into account close relationships (termed relationally autonomous reasons, or RARs). Two longitudinal studies examined the degree to which relational self-construal, RARs, and personally autonomous reasons (PARs) predicted goal attainment. In Study 1, 166 participants rated 7 goals on several goal outcomes at 2 sessions. Results revealed that self-construal was positively associated with RARs and that RARs predicted goal attainment, controlling for PARs. Study 2 (N = 177) added a 3rd follow-up to the Study 1 design, and results showed perceived progress toward one's goals predicted enhanced RARs but not enhanced PARs. Both studies showed that RARs are an effective motivational component in goal pursuit and attainment. 相似文献
110.
Three experiments explored different schedules of retrieval practice in young adults, older adults, and individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer type. In each experiment, an initial acquisition phase was presented in which participants studied or attempted to retrieve response words to cues, followed by a later cued-recall test. Experiment 1 produced a benefit of expanded retrieval over equal-interval retrieval during acquisition, but this benefit was lost in final cued recall. In Experiments 2 and 3, participants received corrective feedback during acquisition and modified spacing schedules. There was again no evidence of a difference between expanded and equal-interval conditions in final cued recall. Discussion focuses on the potential benefits and costs of expanded retrieval on a theoretical and applied level. 相似文献