全文获取类型
收费全文 | 25617篇 |
免费 | 873篇 |
国内免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
26501篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 262篇 |
2018年 | 398篇 |
2017年 | 433篇 |
2016年 | 446篇 |
2015年 | 306篇 |
2014年 | 380篇 |
2013年 | 1977篇 |
2012年 | 752篇 |
2011年 | 793篇 |
2010年 | 520篇 |
2009年 | 482篇 |
2008年 | 696篇 |
2007年 | 718篇 |
2006年 | 628篇 |
2005年 | 563篇 |
2004年 | 545篇 |
2003年 | 525篇 |
2002年 | 586篇 |
2001年 | 786篇 |
2000年 | 789篇 |
1999年 | 613篇 |
1998年 | 311篇 |
1997年 | 267篇 |
1996年 | 255篇 |
1992年 | 500篇 |
1991年 | 448篇 |
1990年 | 489篇 |
1989年 | 447篇 |
1988年 | 424篇 |
1987年 | 428篇 |
1986年 | 440篇 |
1985年 | 455篇 |
1984年 | 379篇 |
1983年 | 331篇 |
1982年 | 282篇 |
1981年 | 246篇 |
1979年 | 413篇 |
1978年 | 297篇 |
1977年 | 279篇 |
1976年 | 248篇 |
1975年 | 355篇 |
1974年 | 388篇 |
1973年 | 364篇 |
1972年 | 333篇 |
1971年 | 288篇 |
1970年 | 260篇 |
1969年 | 326篇 |
1968年 | 361篇 |
1967年 | 324篇 |
1966年 | 327篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
51.
Neuroticism, extraversion, and related traits in adult twins reared apart and reared together 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
N L Pedersen R Plomin G E McClearn L Friberg 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1988,55(6):950-957
The relative importance of genetic and environmental factors for neuroticism, extraversion, impulsivity, and monotony avoidance were estimated in a sample of 99 monozygotic and 229 dizygotic pairs of twins reared apart (TRA) and a matched sample of 160 monozygotic and 212 dizygotic pairs of twins reared together (TRT). The average age was 58.6 (SD = 13.6); 72% of the twins were 50 or older. Model-fitting analyses verified the importance of genetic factors for all four measures; from 23% to 45% of the total variation was attributable to genetic sources. There was considerable evidence that these factors were operating in a nonadditive manner for extraversion and impulsivity. Shared environment accounted for less than 10% of the variance; some evidence for selective placement was found for neuroticism. 相似文献
52.
53.
The kinematic parameters of to and fro forearm movements in the horizontal plane were investigated in 5- to 7-year-old boys. In one condition, the period of the movements was induced by an acoustic stimulus, while the amplitude was left free. In a second condition, the amplitude of the swing was suggested by a visual frame, but the movement was self-paced. In both conditions, the unconstrained variable (amplitude and period, respectively) was found to covary spontaneously with the constrained variable. The results suggest that, within the age range considered here, motor control processes do not conceive of amplitude and velocity as two independent variables. 相似文献
54.
J T Cacioppo J S Martzke R E Petty L G Tassinary 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1988,54(4):592-604
Previous research has demonstrated that mild negative emotional imagery and unpleasant sensory stimuli lead to greater electromyographic activity over the brow muscle region than mild positive imagery and stimuli, even in the absence of significant changes in visceral and general facial EMG activity. Previous research has not addressed whether electromyographic responses over the brow region are a sensitive and specific index of emotions, however, since a multiplicity of events lead to changes in brow activity. In this research, facial electromyographic and audiovisual recordings were obtained while individuals were interviewed about themselves. Afterwards, individuals were asked to describe what they had been thinking of during specific segments of the interview marked by distinctive electromyographic responses over the brow region in the context of ongoing but stable levels of activity elsewhere in the face. The results are interpreted in terms of a continuous flow hypothesis of affect-laden information processing. 相似文献
55.
Perceptual sensitivity to dimensional and similarity relations in free and rule-governed classifications 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Two experiments were conducted to investigate the perceptual primacy of dimensional and similarity relations in the stimulus classifications of younger and older subjects. In Experiment 1, 4- and 10-year-olds were given free classifications in which they could group stimuli according to overall similarity or identities in size, color, or orientation. Both age groups classified stimuli most frequently according to identities on separate dimensions. In Experiment 2, 4-year-olds and adults were given free classifications followed by rule-governed classifications which required them to group stimuli according to specific relations. In the free classifications, a majority of subjects in both age groups classified the stimuli most frequently according to identities on separate dimensions. In the rule-governed classifications, both age groups were more accurate when a single separate relation was required for solution than when overall similarity was required. These results support a differential-sensitivity view of perceptual development, which asserts that individuals at all ages primarily perceive and use separate relations. 相似文献
56.
A D Pick C F Palmer B L Hennessy M G Unze R K Jones R M Richardson 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1988,45(1):28-51
Children's perception of scale and contour in melodies was investigated in five studies. Experimental tasks included judging transposed renditions of melodies (Studies 1 and 3), discriminating between transposed renditions of a melody (Study 2), judging contour-preserving transformations of melodies (Study 4), and judging similarity to a familiar target melody of transformations preserving rhythm or rhythm and contour (Study 5). The first and second studies showed that young children detect key transposition changes even in familiar melodies and they perceive similarity over key transpositions even in unfamiliar melodies. Young children also are sensitive to melodic contour over transformations that preserve it (Study 5), yet they distinguish spontaneously between melodies with the same contour and different intervals (Study 4). The key distance effect reported in the literature did not occur in the tasks of this investigation (Studies 1 and 3), and it may be apparent only for melodies shorter or more impoverished than those used here. 相似文献
57.
58.
Susan D. Holloway Kathleen S. Gorman Bruce Fuller 《International journal of psychology》1988,23(1-6):303-317
The child-rearing beliefs of 32 mothers and 36 day-care providers in Mexico were compared. Day-care providers expected children to master developmental skills at an earlier age than did mothers. Day-care providers more strongly valued the development of independent and cooperative behavior, and placed less importance on obedience. They also reported employing more flexible and nonauthoritarian discipline strategies than did mothers. Mothers and caregivers did not differ in the extent to which they attributed the success of their discipline strategies to their own actions rather than to external factors. Also examined was how mothers' beliefs differed in families characterized by interdependent versus individualistic social structures. In interdependent families, mothers were more likely to believe in later mastery of developmental skills and to make external attributions. These findings suggest that Mexican children experience incongruous social norms as they move between home and day care settings, and that these norms, at least within the home, are associated with the social structural features of the setting. 相似文献
59.
60.