全文获取类型
收费全文 | 35916篇 |
免费 | 654篇 |
国内免费 | 6篇 |
专业分类
36576篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 396篇 |
2019年 | 497篇 |
2018年 | 683篇 |
2017年 | 655篇 |
2016年 | 756篇 |
2015年 | 525篇 |
2014年 | 617篇 |
2013年 | 2805篇 |
2012年 | 1155篇 |
2011年 | 1161篇 |
2010年 | 736篇 |
2009年 | 780篇 |
2008年 | 997篇 |
2007年 | 1033篇 |
2006年 | 905篇 |
2005年 | 784篇 |
2004年 | 758篇 |
2003年 | 727篇 |
2002年 | 755篇 |
2001年 | 1117篇 |
2000年 | 1117篇 |
1999年 | 851篇 |
1998年 | 387篇 |
1997年 | 358篇 |
1996年 | 352篇 |
1995年 | 349篇 |
1992年 | 689篇 |
1991年 | 641篇 |
1990年 | 623篇 |
1989年 | 585篇 |
1988年 | 576篇 |
1987年 | 521篇 |
1986年 | 522篇 |
1985年 | 566篇 |
1984年 | 480篇 |
1983年 | 431篇 |
1982年 | 358篇 |
1979年 | 509篇 |
1978年 | 353篇 |
1977年 | 336篇 |
1976年 | 347篇 |
1975年 | 380篇 |
1974年 | 460篇 |
1973年 | 485篇 |
1972年 | 410篇 |
1971年 | 346篇 |
1969年 | 348篇 |
1968年 | 428篇 |
1967年 | 380篇 |
1966年 | 364篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
911.
912.
913.
Jean Guichard Jacques Pouyaud Cécile de Calan Bernadette Dumora 《Journal of Vocational Behavior》2012,81(1):52-58
Today's wealthy societies are more fluid, varied and complex than they were just a few decades ago. As a consequence, what were “vocational choices” at the beginning of the 20th century now appear as “life designing issues”. In this context, contemporary research stresses the plurality and relative malleability of human subjects as well as their ability to take reflexive stances on their current and past experiences. Fitting in such an epistemology, a self-constructing model is proposed as a basis for a life designing counseling interview. This model describes self-identity as a dynamic system of (past, present and expected) subjective identity forms (SIF), the synthesis and dynamism of which originate in a tension between two kinds of reflexivity. Counseling interviews with emerging adults show that the elicitation of some expected SIF allows them to re-read their current and past experiences from such a perspective and constitutes a compelling incentive to act. 相似文献
914.
Intolerance of uncertainty (IU) is a construct known to influence catastrophic worry and is often observed in Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD). Research into the psychological manifestations of GAD suggests IU is associated with worry, but has not confirmed a causal link. The current study investigated the relationship between catastrophic worry and IU in a non-clinical undergraduate and postgraduate population (n = 46), with a mean age of 26.8 (SD = 5.52 years), where 71.74% were women. Participants received either a high or low IU manipulation, mood was measured throughout the study on 100 point visual analogue scales (VAS), and worry was measured using the catastrophising interview (CI). The high IU group generated significantly more catastrophising steps than the low IU group. Increased levels of sadness and anxiety were observed in the high as compared to the low IU group post IU manipulation, and this difference was maintained throughout the CI interview. A mediation analysis revealed that sadness and anxiety did not significantly mediate the relationship between IU and number of CI steps. These findings have implications for GAD treatment, as they suggest that manipulating IU affects measures of worry and its associated emotional and behavioural symptoms. 相似文献
915.
916.
Unidirectional motion of a uniplanar background induces a codirectional postural sway. It has been shown recently that fixation of a stationary foreground object induces a sway response in the opposite direction (Bronstein & Buckwell, 1997) when the background moves transiently. The present study investigated factors determining this contradirectional postural response. In the experiments presented, center of foot pressure and head displacements were recorded from normal subjects. The subjects faced a visual background of 2 x 3 m, at a distance of 1.5 m, which could be moved parallel to the interaural axis. Results showed that when the visual scene consisted solely of a moving background, the conventional codirectional postural response was elicited. When subjects were asked to fixate an earth-fixed foreground (window frame) placed between them and the moving background, a consistent postural response in the opposite direction to background motion was observed. In addition, we showed that this contradirectional postural response was not transient but was sustained for the 11 sec of background motion. We investigated whether this contradirectional postural response was the consequence of the induced movement of the foreground by background motion. Although induced movement was verbally reported by subjects when viewing an earth-fixed target projected onto the moving background, the contradirectional sway did not occur. These results indicate that foreground-background separation in depth was necessary for the contradirectional postural response to occur rather than induced movement. Another experiment showed that, when the fixated foreground was attached to the head of the observer, the contradirectional sway was not observed and was therefore unrelated to vergence. Finally, results showed that the contradirectional postural response was, in the main, monocularly mediated. We conclude that the direction of the postural sway produced by a moving background in a three-dimensional environment is determined primarily by motion parallax. 相似文献
917.
Schopler J Insko CA Wieselquist J Pemberton M Witcher B Kozar R Roddenberry C Wildschut T 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2001,80(4):632-644
The related goals of the research were to delineate the domain of discontinuity, 1st by demonstrating its occurrence in a nonmatrix situation and, 2nd, by establishing the antecedent outcome conditions necessary for producing a discontinuity effect. The 1st goal was met by designing a mixed motive situation involving the production of origami products. Under these conditions, the magnitude of the discontinuity effect did not significantly differ from that in a matrix-only condition. The 2nd goal was met by a separate experiment that used H. H. Kelley and J. W. Thibaut's (1978) analysis of degree of noncorrespondence of outcomes. This experiment demonstrated that as noncorrespondence increased, so did the rate of competitive responding by groups but not by individuals. This pattern was qualified by an interaction with gender such that competitiveness was more markedly affected by noncorrespondence for groups of women than for groups of men. 相似文献
918.
919.
Research conducted by V. Magley, C. Hulin, L. F. Fitzgerald, and M. DeNardo (1999) has suggested that women who experience sexual harassment report worse outcomes independent of the labeling process. This study replicates and extends that work. Discriminant analyses were conducted on a sample of approximately 28,000 men and women from the military. The authors included variables similar to those used by V. Magley et al., as well as a variety of antecedent variables. Two significant functions were obtained from the discriminant analysis. The 1st function ordered groups according to the frequency of harassment and accounted for substantially more variance than did the 2nd function, which ordered groups according to whether they labeled their experiences as sexual harassment. The overall results from these analyses demonstrate that labeling incidents as sexual harassment is of marginal meaningfulness in terms of job outcomes and antecedents of harassment. 相似文献
920.