首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   452篇
  免费   37篇
  2021年   6篇
  2020年   11篇
  2019年   10篇
  2018年   11篇
  2017年   20篇
  2016年   19篇
  2015年   14篇
  2014年   17篇
  2013年   44篇
  2012年   29篇
  2011年   26篇
  2010年   23篇
  2009年   21篇
  2008年   14篇
  2007年   18篇
  2006年   17篇
  2005年   15篇
  2004年   13篇
  2003年   24篇
  2002年   12篇
  2001年   7篇
  2000年   13篇
  1999年   8篇
  1998年   5篇
  1996年   3篇
  1995年   5篇
  1994年   4篇
  1993年   6篇
  1992年   4篇
  1990年   2篇
  1989年   3篇
  1988年   3篇
  1987年   2篇
  1985年   4篇
  1984年   4篇
  1983年   2篇
  1981年   2篇
  1979年   2篇
  1977年   4篇
  1976年   2篇
  1975年   3篇
  1974年   2篇
  1972年   5篇
  1971年   4篇
  1970年   2篇
  1968年   2篇
  1964年   3篇
  1952年   1篇
  1951年   3篇
  1950年   1篇
排序方式: 共有489条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
Twelve boys and 12 girls at each of 3 ages (4, 5, and 6 years) were tested on three types of conservation judgments (qualitative, quantitative, and equivalence) on both continuous and discontinuous substances. Half of the subjects were provided a memory aid while the other half were not. Conservation ability was determined both with and without verbal justification. The memory aid increased the number of equivalence conservation responses only when verbal justification was not required. The number of subjects conserving on each of the three tasks varied as a function of the scoring criteria used. Type of materials, sex, and age effects were found. The role of transitive inferences and memory in the equivalence conservation task was discussed. Inconsistencies in previous research due to different scoring techniques were noted.  相似文献   
92.
The effects of automated cueing on teacher praise rate was investigated in one special, one fourth-, and one seventh-grade classroom. After establishing baseline praise rates in each of the three classrooms, two methods for increasing teacher praise rate were introduced according to a multiple-baseline design. During the first phase, two teachers were instructed to count and graph their praise rate during each session. All teachers received auditory cues to prompt praising during another phase. In all cases, introduction of cues markedly increased teacher praise rates, but self-recording was relatively ineffective. An analysis of teacher-praise distributions showed that cues closely controlled teacher praise for two of the three teachers.  相似文献   
93.
94.
The basic processing elements (the neurons) in the brain operate in the millisecond range and are about a million times slower than a computer (Feldman, 1985). In order to make up for this lack of computational speed, the brain must use a parallel multisynaptic method to transmit information. This paper presents a parallel processing model of the auditory system; the model accounts for the frequency and the sound-localization information.  相似文献   
95.
The contribution of cognition and affect to evaluations of stereotyped group members was examined. Subjects were American male undergraduates who evaluated a male homosexual or a male heterosexual applicant to a program in elementary education or fine arts. Cognitive measures used to predict evaluations were the discrepancies between stereotype components for the social category and stereotype components for the occupation; affective measures were four dimensions of mood-affect. Results indicated that negative affect predicted evaluations of homosexuals but not heterosexuals. Homosexuals were evaluated less favorably than heterosexuals for both occupations, despite the fact that homosexuals were perceived as less discrepant from occupational members than heterosexuals.  相似文献   
96.
The implicational relations between dispositional levels and relevant categories of behavior were investigated. Observers were asked questions about the extent to which persons occupying moderate or extreme positions on an attribute (dispositional) continuum (a) attempt various behaviors, (b) are potentially able to perform various behaviors, and (c) generally emit those same behaviors. Three determinants—central tendency, ability, and social desirabliity—offer a reasonable account of the observed implicational relations. First, persons with a given disposition are not expected to emit behavior that is widely discrepant with that disposition. Second, ability considerations dictate that persons with skillful dispositions are believed capable of unskillful behavior, but unskillful persons are thought relatively incapable of skillful behavior. Third, persons are believed to more frequently attempt socially desirable than socially undesirable behavior. Also, persons with moderate dispositions are believed to actually emit socially desirable behavior more frequently than socially undesirable behavior. Finally, the determinants noted above may be of differing relevance for morality, ability, and preference attributes.  相似文献   
97.
In the first experiment, refractoriness of a primary arm swing was studied using two movement speeds and three secondary responses (reversal, contralateral and ipsilateral thumb-lift). Pre-movement inter-stimulus intervals were 100, 200 or 300 msec and response-stimulus intervals ranged from zero to 200 msec. Accelerometers provided kinematic data. The second experiment repeated the reversal condition with the addition of EMG analysis.The prediction that the maximal speed condition would show a general intensification of control processes, and thus less refractoriness as compared to submaximal speed, was upheld for all response conditions. The findings also supported the prediction that the pattern of refractoriness depends on the functional relationships of the muscles concerned.The EMG analysis revealed that while the sequencing of muscle action was unchanged with movement speed, its phasing characterized the shifts in response metrics. The overall findings emphasize the need to view the refractoriness phenomenon in the context of anatomical and mechanical consequences of force changes in controlling limb movement as they interact with intentional command.  相似文献   
98.
5 normal speakers and 5 speakers with congenital upper motor-neuron damage repeatedly spoke a three-work sentence stressing the first, second, or third words. Fundamental frequency was measured for each word and over each sentence. Analysis showed that the spastics' mean fundamental frequency was significantly higher than the normals', regardless of utterance. Also, spastics were more restricted in their prosodic maneuvering above this average fundamental frequency. Despite this finding, the spastics were capable of producing consistent stress patterns with normal variability.  相似文献   
99.
100.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号