首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   949篇
  免费   36篇
  国内免费   2篇
  2023年   9篇
  2020年   12篇
  2019年   26篇
  2018年   22篇
  2017年   21篇
  2016年   39篇
  2015年   16篇
  2014年   30篇
  2013年   120篇
  2012年   51篇
  2011年   36篇
  2010年   29篇
  2009年   23篇
  2008年   29篇
  2007年   32篇
  2006年   25篇
  2005年   24篇
  2004年   27篇
  2003年   19篇
  2002年   18篇
  2001年   16篇
  2000年   16篇
  1999年   18篇
  1998年   8篇
  1995年   11篇
  1994年   9篇
  1993年   9篇
  1992年   9篇
  1991年   8篇
  1990年   10篇
  1989年   12篇
  1988年   12篇
  1987年   12篇
  1986年   9篇
  1985年   13篇
  1984年   9篇
  1983年   9篇
  1981年   7篇
  1980年   9篇
  1979年   10篇
  1978年   9篇
  1977年   10篇
  1976年   10篇
  1975年   11篇
  1974年   9篇
  1973年   9篇
  1972年   8篇
  1971年   9篇
  1968年   10篇
  1967年   7篇
排序方式: 共有987条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
911.
Ohne Zusammenfassung Mit 57 Abbildungen. Dissertation der Naturwissenschaftlichen Fakult?t Frankfurt a. M. — Diese Untersuchung behandelt nur den ersten Teil des Gesamtproblems, die Frage der anschaulichen Zusammenhangsverh?ltnisse. Die Frage der anschaulichen Form im engeren Sinn soll in einer besonderen Arbeit behandelt werden.  相似文献   
912.
Solving a conflict between two response options in an interference task has been found to increase control in a subsequent conflict situation. The present research examined whether such conflict adaptation persists in the presence of distractors that have motivational relevance and are therefore competing for attentional resources (i.e. they signal opportunities for monetary gains or losses contingent on overall task performance). In an adjusted flanker task, motivational (versus neutral versus no) distractors were presented together with the current trial while the previous trial never included any distractor. Accumulated evidence across three studies showed that motivational distractors reduced the conflict adaptation effect. This was found irrespective of the location at which the distractor occurred (Study 1), and independent of its valence (i.e. reward or loss, Study 2). Study 3 and a merged data analysis ruled out low-level alternative explanations. In line with a dual competition account (Pessoa, L. (2009). How do emotion and motivation direct executive control? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(4), 160–166. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.006), our results show that conflict adaptation is not fully protected in the presence of motivational distractors. We discuss whether this should be interpreted as a limitation, or as reflecting the flexibility of the control system in dealing with motivationally relevant information.  相似文献   
913.
914.
915.
This study investigated the correspondence between parents' and daughters' beliefs about sexual aggression and gender roles. The relationship between a woman's attitudes and her personal experiences with sexual victimization was also examined. The participants were 236 female undergraduates, 148 mothers, and 110 fathers. One hundred-three matching triads were collected. Participants evaluated victim responsibility for written scenarios depicting a date-rape victim. Information about gender-role attitudes, perceived family communication, and previous sexual experiences was also collected. Results indicated that daughter–mother, daughter–father, and mother–father dyads shared attitudes about gender roles and beliefs about victim responsibility. Parental attitudes also predicted daughters' attitudes, but family communication did not moderate the relationship between parental attitudes and daughters' attitudes. Mothers' and daughters' experiences of coerced sex were not associated. A relationship between attitudes and beliefs and experiences of coerced sex emerged only for mothers. Mothers with a history of coerced sex adhered to more traditional gender-role attitudes and assigned more responsibility to the date-rape victim. The need for further research in the area of familial attitudes about rape is discussed.  相似文献   
916.
917.
918.
919.
920.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号