全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2407篇 |
免费 | 25篇 |
专业分类
2432篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 16篇 |
2021年 | 29篇 |
2020年 | 44篇 |
2019年 | 35篇 |
2018年 | 70篇 |
2017年 | 66篇 |
2016年 | 80篇 |
2015年 | 46篇 |
2014年 | 71篇 |
2013年 | 273篇 |
2012年 | 126篇 |
2011年 | 97篇 |
2010年 | 75篇 |
2009年 | 64篇 |
2008年 | 69篇 |
2007年 | 97篇 |
2006年 | 65篇 |
2005年 | 90篇 |
2004年 | 75篇 |
2003年 | 52篇 |
2002年 | 65篇 |
2001年 | 44篇 |
2000年 | 40篇 |
1999年 | 53篇 |
1998年 | 24篇 |
1997年 | 18篇 |
1996年 | 19篇 |
1995年 | 24篇 |
1994年 | 24篇 |
1993年 | 24篇 |
1992年 | 29篇 |
1991年 | 27篇 |
1990年 | 24篇 |
1989年 | 20篇 |
1988年 | 21篇 |
1987年 | 22篇 |
1986年 | 16篇 |
1985年 | 17篇 |
1983年 | 17篇 |
1982年 | 17篇 |
1981年 | 18篇 |
1980年 | 22篇 |
1979年 | 27篇 |
1978年 | 15篇 |
1976年 | 19篇 |
1975年 | 15篇 |
1974年 | 29篇 |
1973年 | 26篇 |
1971年 | 20篇 |
1969年 | 15篇 |
排序方式: 共有2432条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
81.
82.
83.
We measured driving performance (lane-keeping errors, driving times, and glances away from the road scene) in a video driving simulator for 24 volunteers who each drove alone on a 10.6-km multicurved course while simultaneously placing calls on a mobile phone subscribed to a voice-activated dialing system. Driving performance also was measured for the same distance while participants manually dialed phone numbers and while they drove without dialing. There were 22% fewer lane-keeping errors (p<.01) and 56% fewer glances away from the road scene (p<.01) when they used voice-activated dialing as compared to manual dialing. Significantly longer driving times in both of the dialing conditions as compared to the No Dialing condition are discussed in terms of the hypothesis that drivers decrease driving speed to compensate for the demands of the secondary phone tasks. 相似文献
84.
Raymond J. Taylor 《Contemporary Family Therapy》2002,24(3):475-481
This is a case study of a family with a child (age eight) with reactive attachment disorder and the subsequent individual and family therapy. Treatment of choice for the child was Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing (EMDR) and supportive educational counseling for the parents and family. Qualitative evaluation of the process demonstrated that the parents observed an instant change in the child's attitude. The child reported that she felt better about family, school, and truthfulness, and stated about the therapy: It opened a window for me. A 12-month evaluation demonstrated continued positive effects. 相似文献
85.
Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight 总被引:29,自引:0,他引:29
Taylor SE Klein LC Lewis BP Gruenewald TL Gurung RA Updegraff JA 《Psychological review》2000,107(3):411-429
The human stress response has been characterized, both physiologically and behaviorally, as "fight-or-flight." Although fight-or-flight may characterize the primary physiological responses to stress for both males and females, we propose that, behaviorally, females' responses are more marked by a pattern of "tend-and-befriend." Tending involves nurturant activities designed to protect the self and offspring that promote safety and reduce distress; befriending is the creation and maintenance of social networks that may aid in this process. The biobehavioral mechanism that underlies the tend-and-befriend pattern appears to draw on the attachment-caregiving system, and neuroendocrine evidence from animal and human studies suggests that oxytocin, in conjunction with female reproductive hormones and endogenous opioid peptide mechanisms, may be at its core. This previously unexplored stress regulatory system has manifold implications for the study of stress. 相似文献
86.
An "aha" effect in memory was first reported by Auble, Franks, and Soraci (1979). They demonstrated that recall was greater for sentences that were initially incomprehensible but which were eventually comprehended, as compared with sentences that were understood from the outset. The present studies extend this "aha" effect to memory for pictorial stimuli. In Experiment 1, a recall advantage for pictures encoded by connecting the dots as compared with those encoded by tracing or visual scanning occurred only in the absence of foreknowledge of the picture (i.e., an "aha" effect). In Experiment 2, we replicated this finding and obtained evidence that conceptually based, verbal foreknowledge does not function in a similar manner as does pictorial foreknowledge in suppressing the "aha" recall advantage. These results place important constraints on previous research on generation effects for visual stimuli and attest to the cross-modal generalizability of the "aha" effect. 相似文献
87.
Gellis LA Lichstein KL Scarinci IC Durrence HH Taylor DJ Bush AJ Riedel BW 《Journal of abnormal psychology》2005,114(1):111-118
This investigation compared the likelihood of insomnia and insomnia-related health consequences among individuals of different socioeconomic status. A random-digit dialing procedure was used to recruit at least 50 men and 50 women in each age decade from 20 to 80+ years old. Participants completed 2 weeks of sleep diaries as well as questionnaires related to fatigue, sleepiness, and psychological distress. Socioeconomic status was measured by education status assessed at 3 different levels: individual, household, and community. Results indicated that individuals of lower individual and household education were significantly more likely to experience insomnia even after researchers accounted for ethnicity, gender, and age. Additionally, individuals with fewer years of education, particularly those who had dropped out of high school, experienced greater subjective impairment because of their insomnia. 相似文献
88.
Klapp ST Haas BW 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2005,31(1):193-209
A pattern-masked arrow negatively biased the "free choice" between 2 manual responses or between 2 vocal responses. This apparently nonconscious influence occurred only when the free-choice trials were intermixed randomly with other trials that terminated in fully visible arrows, which directed a response of the same modality (manual vs. vocal) as that involved in the free-choice test trials. This indicates that recent conscious processing of the association between specific stimuli and specific responses is needed to activate the nonconscious influence of masked arrows on response selection. Because this influence occurred only when a concrete association was activated, it appears not to be based on deep comprehension of the stimuli and instead is attributable to simple stimulus-response bonds. 相似文献
89.
90.
In this comment, J. S. Ablon and C. Marci argue that focusing on the empirical validation of manualized treatment packages misses important information about what is efficacious about a given treatment. Psychotherapy process has demonstrated that treatments may promote change in ways other than their underlying theories claim. Manualized therapies may appear distinct despite important similarities in dyadic interaction. These functional similarities in the emergent transactional process between therapist and patient may help explain the difficulty demonstrating differential outcomes across brands of brief therapy. Rather than focus on treatment packages targeting patient symptomatology, the authors recommend a shift in focus to the empirical validation of change processes coconstructed by therapist and patient in naturalistic settings. 相似文献