首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   717篇
  免费   22篇
  2022年   4篇
  2021年   4篇
  2020年   6篇
  2019年   9篇
  2018年   19篇
  2017年   14篇
  2016年   23篇
  2015年   10篇
  2014年   21篇
  2013年   88篇
  2012年   32篇
  2011年   23篇
  2010年   17篇
  2009年   17篇
  2008年   22篇
  2007年   34篇
  2006年   17篇
  2005年   28篇
  2004年   28篇
  2003年   22篇
  2002年   26篇
  2001年   15篇
  2000年   10篇
  1999年   17篇
  1998年   9篇
  1997年   8篇
  1996年   7篇
  1995年   6篇
  1994年   7篇
  1993年   9篇
  1992年   11篇
  1991年   6篇
  1990年   9篇
  1989年   5篇
  1988年   8篇
  1985年   4篇
  1984年   6篇
  1983年   10篇
  1982年   5篇
  1981年   12篇
  1980年   12篇
  1979年   9篇
  1978年   6篇
  1977年   6篇
  1976年   9篇
  1975年   7篇
  1974年   13篇
  1973年   11篇
  1971年   6篇
  1970年   5篇
排序方式: 共有739条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
231.
Anstis S  Gori S  Wehrhahn C 《Perception》2007,36(5):791-794
Gori and Stubbs (2006 Perception 35 1573-1577) have published some visual illusions elicited in observers who fixate very blurred disks while they move their head towards and away from them. We interpret these illusions as afterimages. We support this with examples of eccentric, colored, and striped afterimages.  相似文献   
232.
The present study explores the factors underlying competence in a difficult consonant deletion task, using a sample of nine-year-old children whose development had been followed from the age of four. In particular it examines the factors governing use of phonological and orthographic strategies in the deletion task, and the relations between strategy use, current reading and spelling ability, and earlier phonological awareness skills. Processes in consonant deletion are then mapped on to current dual-route models of reading and spelling.  相似文献   
233.
Editorial     
Stuart Mews 《Religion》1985,15(1):1-2
  相似文献   
234.
235.
Informed broadly by the theory of planned behaviour, this study used qualitative methodology to understand Australian adults' sun‐protective decisions. Forty‐two adults participated in focus groups where they discussed behavioural (advantages and disadvantages), normative (important referents), and control (barriers and facilitators) beliefs, as well as potential social influences and images of tanned and non‐tanned people. Responses were analysed using the consensual qualitative research approach to determine the dominant themes. Themes of fashion and comfort were prominent, the important role of friends and family in sun safe decision‐making was highlighted, as was the availability of sun‐protective measures (e.g., in an accessible place or in the environment). Additional themes included the need to model sound sun‐protective behaviours to (current and future) children, the emphasis on personal choice and personal responsibility to be sun safe, and the influence of Australian identity and culture on tanning and socially acceptable forms of sun protection. These beliefs can be used to inform interventions and public health campaigns targeting sun safety among Australians, a population with the highest skin cancer incidence in the world.  相似文献   
236.
237.
We investigated for the first time whether the principles of specificity could be extended to the psychological construct of anxiety and whether any benefits of practicing with anxiety are dependent on the amount of exposure and timing of that exposure in relation to where in learning the exposure occurs. In Experiment 1, novices practiced a discrete golf-putting task in one of four groups: all practice trials under anxiety (anxiety), non-anxiety (control), or a combination of these two (i.e., the first half of practice under anxiety before changing to non-anxiety conditions, anxiety-control, or the reverse of this, control-anxiety). Following acquisition, all groups were transferred to an anxiety condition. Results revealed a significant acquisition-to-transfer decrement in performance between acquisition and transfer for the control group only. In Experiment 2, novices practiced a complex rock climbing task in one of the four groups detailed above, before being transferred to both a high-anxiety condition and a low-anxiety condition (the ordering of these was counterbalanced across participants). Performance in anxiety transfer was greater following practice with anxiety compared to practice without anxiety. However, these benefits were influenced by the timing of anxiety exposure since performance was greatest when exposure to anxiety occurred in the latter half of acquisition. In the low-anxiety transfer test, performance was lowest for those who had practiced with anxiety only, thus providing support for the specificity of practice hypothesis. Results demonstrate that the specificity of learning principle can be extended to include the psychological construct of anxiety. Furthermore, the specificity advantage appears dependent on its timing in the learning process.  相似文献   
238.
239.
240.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号