首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1229篇
  免费   45篇
  国内免费   1篇
  2021年   11篇
  2019年   19篇
  2018年   28篇
  2017年   22篇
  2016年   38篇
  2015年   17篇
  2014年   26篇
  2013年   148篇
  2012年   53篇
  2011年   34篇
  2010年   28篇
  2009年   28篇
  2008年   35篇
  2007年   56篇
  2006年   35篇
  2005年   40篇
  2004年   47篇
  2003年   40篇
  2002年   40篇
  2001年   20篇
  2000年   16篇
  1999年   26篇
  1998年   15篇
  1997年   13篇
  1996年   16篇
  1995年   10篇
  1994年   12篇
  1993年   11篇
  1992年   18篇
  1991年   11篇
  1990年   11篇
  1989年   15篇
  1988年   15篇
  1987年   9篇
  1985年   13篇
  1984年   15篇
  1983年   17篇
  1982年   12篇
  1981年   19篇
  1980年   26篇
  1979年   18篇
  1978年   11篇
  1977年   15篇
  1976年   16篇
  1975年   16篇
  1974年   21篇
  1973年   18篇
  1972年   10篇
  1971年   11篇
  1967年   7篇
排序方式: 共有1275条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
The role of knowledge of the reversibility of reversible figures was tested in four experiments. Two ambiguous figures, the vase-face figure and a depth-reversing pyramid-hallway figure were shown to high school students. In the Uninformed condition, subjects were not told that the figures were reversible. A sampling procedure was used in which subjects reported what they perceived at 5-sec intervals. Viewing durations of up to 3 min were used, and approximately half of all subjects did not reverse at all during the uninformed condition, whereas virtually all subjects reversed quickly and frequently once they knew that the figures were reversible. These results are not consistent with neural fatigue models of perceptual reversal.  相似文献   
62.
Four experiments investigated the effects of labelling on the encoding and recognition of schematic faces. In Experiments I and II, hard-to-label (H) faces were recognized better after labelling than after observing, whereas easy-to-label (E) faces were not significantly affected. However, E faces were recognized better after labelling when subjects were instructed to attend carefully to all features of the faces during viewing. In Experiments III and IV, which dealt with each kind of face separately, both E and H faces were recognized better after labelling than after observing. An additional improvement in recognition all of faces was found when labelling subjects knew which label was relevant on each recognition trial and were instructed to use it. It was concluded that (a) a verbal label functions mainly to direct attention to specific facial features during viewing, (b)the effect of the label on recognition is positive or negative depending on whether or not it directs attention to features which are functional for the recognition test, (c) the label also forms part of the representation of the face in memory, and (d) a label may serve as a mediator at the time of testing, but only in the presence of specific cues.  相似文献   
63.
The “oblique effect” was studied using an embedded figures task in which complex patterns and hidden forms were rotated into different orientations. One set of stimuli (Series I) used patterns consisting either entirely of depending on rotation) horizontal and vertical line segments or entirely of obliques. A second set of stimuli (Series II) used patterns composed of horizontal, vertical, and oblique segments in equal proportions. With Series I, both form and complex pattern orientations changed with rotation. In Series II, rotation changed the orientation of the hidden forms, but not the relative proportion of contour orientations in the complex patterns. Significantly longer search times were required to find forms embedded within the obliquely oriented patterns of Series I. There were no significant differences in Series II. These results are discussed in terms of the role contextual contours may play in the detection of differently oriented form.  相似文献   
64.
A projector and cylindrical lens are used for displaying gratings and for simple demonstrations of Weber’s law, Fechner’s law, simultaneous contrast, and Fourier synthesis effects in vision.  相似文献   
65.
66.
67.
Three theories of analogy have been proposed that are supported by computational models and data from experiments on human analogical abilities. In this article we show how these theories can be unified within a common metatheoretical framework that distinguishes among levels of informational, behavioral, and hardware constraints. This framework clarifies the distinctions among three computational models in the literature: the Analogical Constraint Mapping Engine (ACME), the Structure-Mapping Engine (SME), and the Incremental Analogy Machine (IAM). We then go on to develop a methodology for the comparative testing of these models. In two different manipulations of an analogical mapping task we compare the results of computational experiments with these models against the results of psychological experiments. In the first experiment we show that increasing the number of similar elements in two analogical domains decreases the response time taken to reach the correct mapping for an analogy problem. In the second psychological experiment we find that the order in which the elements of the two domains are presented has significant facilitative effects on the ease of analogical mapping. Of the three models, only IAM embodies behavioral constraints and predicts both of these results. Finally, the immediate implications of these results for analogy research are discussed, along with the wider implications the research has for cognitive science methodology.  相似文献   
68.
69.
70.
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology - The purpose of this study was to identify items from the Revised Children’s Anxiety and Depression Scale – RCADS-C/P that provided a...  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号