首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   38510篇
  免费   607篇
  国内免费   2篇
  39119篇
  2020年   437篇
  2019年   551篇
  2018年   723篇
  2017年   794篇
  2016年   807篇
  2015年   564篇
  2014年   689篇
  2013年   2912篇
  2012年   1255篇
  2011年   1248篇
  2010年   830篇
  2009年   768篇
  2008年   1118篇
  2007年   1134篇
  2006年   1007篇
  2005年   891篇
  2004年   834篇
  2003年   791篇
  2002年   809篇
  2001年   1163篇
  2000年   1110篇
  1999年   867篇
  1998年   377篇
  1997年   373篇
  1995年   355篇
  1993年   349篇
  1992年   731篇
  1991年   648篇
  1990年   701篇
  1989年   604篇
  1988年   639篇
  1987年   600篇
  1986年   608篇
  1985年   546篇
  1984年   505篇
  1983年   483篇
  1982年   351篇
  1979年   569篇
  1978年   399篇
  1977年   345篇
  1975年   450篇
  1974年   495篇
  1973年   529篇
  1972年   399篇
  1971年   389篇
  1970年   351篇
  1969年   401篇
  1968年   472篇
  1967年   429篇
  1966年   354篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
881.
A theory of intelligence is couched in stimulus-response terms, bridging the gap between S-R and cognitive psychology. The chief theoretical concepts are span ability (a capacity notion), response string (sequentially cued responses), and complexity of stimulus control (task complexity). Span is equated with the ability to respond appropriately when several cues are conjunctively relevant for correct performance (complex stimulus control). So defined, span is consistently and broadly related to many aspects of intelligent behavior.  相似文献   
882.
The use of complementary metal oxide semiconductor (CMOS) technology to produce fixed-ratio and random-ratio schedules is described. Such a technology implies several advantages over traditional methods of construction, especially with respect to cost and complexity. Suitable interfaces for linking with everyday laboratory equipment are also described.  相似文献   
883.
884.
Ten male and 10 female subjects, half of whom were classified as “heavy” and half as “light” drinkers, ingested a 4-1 combination of tonic water and 80-proof vodka in an amount calculated to produce a state of moderate intoxication (average estimated peak blood alcohol level=.075). This resulted in an increased sensitivity (latency, magnitude, and duration) to the calorically induced “oculogyral illusion” (OGI) relative to a placebo (tonic water) condition. “Heavy” and “light” drinkers did not differ significantly on any of the measures. It was speculated that the alcoholic enhancement of the OGI was due either to a decreased ability to suppress vestibular nystagmus when attempting to fixate a visual stimulus or to the expenditure of extra-normal effort in order to maintain fixation.  相似文献   
885.
Experienced observers were asked to identify, in a four-level 2AFC situation, the longer of two unfilled time intervals, each of which was marked by a pair of 20-msec acoustic pulses. When all the markers were identical, high-level (186-dB SPL) bursts of coherently gated sinusoids or bursts of band-limited Gaussian noise, a change in the spectrum of the markers generally did not affect performance. On the other hand, for 1-kHz tone-burst markers, intensity decreases below 25 dB SL were accompanied by sizable deterioration of the discrimination performance, especially at short (25-msec) base intervals. Similarly large changes in performance were observed also when the two tonal markers of each interval were made very dissimilar from each other, either in frequency (frequency difference larger than 1 octave) or in intensity (level of the first marker at least 45 dB below the level of the second marker). Time-difference thresholds in these two latter cases were found to be nonmonotonically related to the base interval, the minima occurring between 40- and 80-msec onset separations.  相似文献   
886.
Reaction times were measured in a task which requires the subject to search a brief visual array for a critical letter embedded in a row of background letters. In Experiment 1, the position of a critical letter in an instructed reading order and the size of a set of memorized letters were varied. Mean reaction time increased monotonically with distance of the critical letter from the beginning of the instructed reading path in the display. The variables reading position and memory set size were additive in their effect on mean reaction time. Data from a second experiment in which the retinal location of the critical letter and its reading position were varied showed that both reading position and retinal location influenced mean reaction time, but the effect of reading position on reaction time was greater. These variables interacted.  相似文献   
887.
888.
889.
890.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号