首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   16694篇
  免费   351篇
  国内免费   1篇
  17046篇
  2019年   226篇
  2018年   261篇
  2017年   224篇
  2016年   269篇
  2015年   181篇
  2014年   266篇
  2013年   1300篇
  2012年   450篇
  2011年   500篇
  2010年   297篇
  2009年   319篇
  2008年   426篇
  2007年   454篇
  2006年   429篇
  2005年   351篇
  2004年   376篇
  2003年   338篇
  2002年   340篇
  2001年   458篇
  2000年   515篇
  1999年   345篇
  1998年   209篇
  1997年   156篇
  1996年   168篇
  1995年   166篇
  1992年   277篇
  1991年   286篇
  1990年   296篇
  1989年   277篇
  1988年   268篇
  1987年   236篇
  1986年   247篇
  1985年   260篇
  1984年   192篇
  1983年   211篇
  1982年   159篇
  1980年   156篇
  1979年   250篇
  1978年   220篇
  1977年   156篇
  1975年   215篇
  1974年   245篇
  1973年   268篇
  1972年   225篇
  1971年   212篇
  1970年   163篇
  1969年   196篇
  1968年   248篇
  1967年   241篇
  1966年   209篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
In 2 experiments, the researcher investigated the social desirability of different love styles (Eros, Ludus, Storge, Pragma, Mania, and Agape). In Experiment 1, the Marlowe-Crowne measure of social desirability (D. P. Crowne & D. Marlowe, 1960) was correlated negatively with possessive, dependent (Mania) love styles in both men and women. In men, social desirability was correlated positively with romantic, passionate love (Eros) and game-playing love (Ludus), but negatively with all-giving, selfless love (Agape). In women, social desirability was correlated positively with Agape, but negatively with Ludus. In Experiment 2, the researcher replicated these findings using an impression management manipulation (good, bad, and honest responding). The gender differences in social desirability of different love styles are explained in terms of traditional and nontraditional gender role socialization.  相似文献   
992.
Book Review     
Arnheim, Rudolf, “Picasso's Guernica, The Genesis of a Painting,” University of California Press, Berkeley, California, 1962. Pp. 139, $8.50. Reviewed by Irving R. Stone  相似文献   
993.
994.
The influences of different action-outcome scenarios on children's evaluative judgments and inferences of outcome intentionality were assessed. One hundred forty-five kindergartners, 2nd graders, and 4th graders heard 4 stories about child actors who engaged in 1 action or 3 equifinal actions and caused a positive or negative outcome. The stories made no mention of the actors' anticipated outcome so that we could assess the children's inferences of whether the actors wanted and had tried to cause the outcome. Children also rated their liking for the actors and the actors' morality. Children's moral and liking judgments were not significantly differentiated by action condition. However, actors who caused positive outcomes received favorable liking and moral judgments, and actors who caused negative outcomes received neutral liking and moral judgments. Children's intentionality inferences varied by the actors' actions and were moderated by outcome valence. The authors discuss children's apparent use of the valence rule when inferring intentionality and their reluctance to judge harshly actors who cause negative outcomes when not privy to the actors' intentions.  相似文献   
995.
Forty subjects took part in a one-handed catching task in which the period for which the mechanically projected tennis ball was illuminated in flight was varied systematically. Additionally, they were tested for (a) static visual acuity and (b) dynamic visual acuity, in which angular velocity was varied. As expected, both viewing period in the catching task and angular velocity in the acuity task were significant variables in performance. Correlation and principal-components analyses confirmed the findings of a previous experiment in that the correlated static visual acuity tasks were unrelated to both dynamic visual acuity (even when angular velocity was only 75°/sec) and catching performance. Further, dynamic acuity and catching were related under the majority of the combinations, and most frequently at the highest angular velocity, a fact which suggested that the dynamic element in both tasks is the common factor.  相似文献   
996.
997.
998.
999.
1000.
This investigation examined the association between ethnic identity centrality and peer status for African American adolescents who represented a sizable proportion, yet numerical minority within a high school context. Initial analyses indicated that a traditional sociometric nomination procedure did not adequately characterize peer status for African American adolescents. A modified nomination procedure varying the ethnicity of nominators yielded measures of African American adolescents' peer acceptance/rejection and perceived popularity as rated by African American or European American peers. Results suggest that high levels of peer acceptance and popularity as rated by African American peers were associated with high ethnic identity centrality; however, acceptance and popularity as rated by European American peers were unrelated to ethnic identity centrality among African American adolescents. Findings suggest the importance of examining peer status of African American adolescents with consideration given to ethnicity as a relevant context.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号