全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1028篇 |
免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
1035篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 7篇 |
2022年 | 9篇 |
2021年 | 17篇 |
2020年 | 16篇 |
2019年 | 14篇 |
2018年 | 25篇 |
2017年 | 35篇 |
2016年 | 27篇 |
2015年 | 10篇 |
2014年 | 14篇 |
2013年 | 98篇 |
2012年 | 28篇 |
2011年 | 38篇 |
2010年 | 20篇 |
2009年 | 29篇 |
2008年 | 31篇 |
2007年 | 30篇 |
2006年 | 24篇 |
2005年 | 23篇 |
2004年 | 40篇 |
2003年 | 20篇 |
2002年 | 24篇 |
2001年 | 26篇 |
2000年 | 25篇 |
1999年 | 39篇 |
1998年 | 26篇 |
1997年 | 17篇 |
1996年 | 15篇 |
1995年 | 21篇 |
1994年 | 20篇 |
1993年 | 21篇 |
1992年 | 18篇 |
1991年 | 19篇 |
1990年 | 15篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 7篇 |
1987年 | 8篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 9篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1980年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 9篇 |
1975年 | 9篇 |
1974年 | 14篇 |
1973年 | 8篇 |
1972年 | 8篇 |
1970年 | 9篇 |
1968年 | 11篇 |
1967年 | 7篇 |
1966年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有1035条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
991.
LaRue RH Stewart V Piazza CC Volkert VM Patel MR Zeleny J 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2011,44(4):719-735
Given the effectiveness of putative escape extinction as treatment for feeding problems, it is surprising that little is known about the effects of escape as reinforcement for appropriate eating during treatment. In the current investigation, we examined the effectiveness of escape as reinforcement for mouth clean (a product measure of swallowing), escape as reinforcement for mouth clean plus escape extinction (EE), and EE alone as treatment for the food refusal of 5 children. Results were similar to those of previous studies, in that reinforcement alone did not result in increases in mouth clean or decreases in inappropriate behavior (e.g., Piazza, Patel, Gulotta, Sevin, & Layer, 2003). Increases in mouth clean and decreases in inappropriate behavior occurred when the therapist implemented EE independent of the presence or absence of reinforcement. Results are discussed in terms of the role of negative reinforcement in the etiology and treatment of feeding problems. 相似文献
992.
Nikos Ntoumanis Laura C. Healy Constantine Sedikides Joan Duda Brandon Stewart Alison Smith Johanna Bond 《Journal of personality》2014,82(3):225-236
No prior research has examined how motivation for goal striving influences persistence in the face of increasing goal difficulty. This research examined the role of self‐reported (Study 1) and primed (Study 2) autonomous and controlled motives in predicting objectively assessed persistence during the pursuit of an increasingly difficult goal. In Study 1, 100 British athletes (64 males; Mage = 19.89 years, SDage = 2.43) pursued a goal of increasing difficulty on a cycle ergometer. In Study 2, 90 British athletes (43 males; Mage = 19.63 years, SDage = 1.14) engaged in the same task, but their motivation was primed by asking them to observe a video of an actor describing her or his involvement in an unrelated study. In Study 1, self‐reported autonomous goal motives predicted goal persistence via challenge appraisals and task‐based coping. In contrast, controlled goal motives predicted threat appraisals and disengagement coping, which, in turn, was a negative predictor of persistence. In Study 2, primed autonomous (compared to controlled) goal motives predicted greater persistence, positive affect, and future interest for task engagement. The findings underscore the importance of autonomous motivation for behavioral investment in the face of increased goal difficulty. 相似文献
993.
994.
When market changes alter what product attributes are deemed important, consumers may intentionally try to forget old product information in an attempt to remember new product information. In Experiment 1, the authors demonstrated that intentional forgetting of this nature temporarily inhibits retrieval of old product information and leads to a benefit to memory for new product information. The results show that, after a short delay, benefits continue in the absence of costs, which is supportive of a multiple-process account of intentional forgetting. Experiment 2 extends these effects using an advertising message to stimulate forgetting. Across both experiments, results also show that brand preference is based on learning of new attribute information. 相似文献
995.
This study assessed psychological adjustment and quality of life relative to population-based norms and knowledge about hereditary
hemochromatosis in a sample of 101 patients who attended a hemochromatosis clinic. Participants were assessed prior to their
clinic visit, and two weeks and 12 months after attendance, using self-administered questionnaires. Mean Mental Health Component
Scores from the Medical Outcomes Study 36-Item Short-Form Health Survey (SF-36) (45.3, 95% CI 43.2, 47.4) were as compromised
as those found amongst stroke victims (45.9, 95% CI 42.8, 49.0) who had participated in a national health survey. Recall of
the genetic testing result was less than optimal, in that only 69.3% of those with genetic testing results knew whether they
carried one or two mutations. This study demonstrates that patients would benefit from routine assessment of psychological
distress and referral to mental health professionals of those whose levels of distress suggest a need for clinical intervention.
Results also show that patients may benefit from strategies aimed at improving recall of genetic testing results. 相似文献
996.
The Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (MFQ) is examined for its utility in screening youth in juvenile justice settings for depression. In a cross-sectional study conducted at King County Juvenile Detention Center, a representative sample of 228 detained adolescents complete structured assessments, including the MFQ and the Massachusetts Youth Screening Instrument. Fifty youth also complete the Voice-Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children. Internal reliability coefficient for the MFQ short form (SMFQ) is = .87. Factor analysis produces a unifactorial scale with item loadings of .43 to .78. At SMFQ cutoff > or = 10, sensitivity and specificity are optimized at 1.00/0.79. Prevalence of major depressive disorder is estimated at 32.1% (95% Confidence Interval = 25.3% to 39.2%). The SMFQ shows potential for depression screening of detained adolescents. 相似文献
997.
998.
Stewart Shapiro 《逻辑史和逻辑哲学》2013,34(1-2):209-230
This paper focuses on two notions of effectiveness which are not treated in detail elsewhere. Unlike the standard computability notion, which is a property of functions themselves, both notions of effectiveness are properties of interpreted linguistic presentations of functions. It is shown that effectiveness is epistemically at least as basic as computability in the sense that decisions about computability normally involve judgments concerning effectiveness. There are many occurrences of the present notions in the writings of logicians; moreover, consideration of these notions can contribute to the clarification and, perhaps, solution of various philosophical problems, confusions and disputes. 相似文献
999.
1000.
Destin N. Stewart 《Journal of aggression, maltreatment & trauma》2013,22(8):853-874
This study examined the impact of a rape complainant's willingness to ingest a chemical substance and the type of ingested substance on the decisions of 229 mock jurors. Jurors were influenced by the type of substance used by the complainant, the complainant's willingness to use a substance, and rape myths. The complainant's ingestion of alcohol (compared to gamma-hydroxybutrate [GHB] and marijuana) led to the highest guilt ratings for the defendant. The complainant who willingly ingested a substance was viewed as less credible and more to blame for the rape compared to one who unwillingly used a substance. The complainant was perceived as the most credible when she unwillingly ingested GHB or marijuana. Jurors high in rape myth acceptance gave lower ratings of guilt to the defendant compared to jurors with lower rape myth acceptance. Overall, the results highlight several juror and case factors that might bias jurors in actual rape trials. 相似文献